Distributional social semantics: Inferring word meanings from communication patterns
Tài liệu tham khảo
Abbot-Smith, 2006, Exemplar-learning and schematization in a usage-based account of syntactic acquisition, The Linguistic Review, 23, 275, 10.1515/TLR.2006.011
Adelman, 2006, Contextual diversity, not word frequency, determines word-naming and lexical decision times, Psychological Science, 17, 814, 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2006.01787.x
Adelman, 2008, Modeling lexical decision: The form of frequency and diversity effects, Psychological Review, 115, 214, 10.1037/0033-295X.115.1.214
Andrews, 2009, Integrating experiential and distributional data to learn semantic representations, Psychological Review, 116, 463, 10.1037/a0016261
Balota, 2004, Visual word recognition of single-syllable words, Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 133, 283, 10.1037/0096-3445.133.2.283
Balota, 2007, The English Lexicon Project, Behavior Research Methods, 39, 445, 10.3758/BF03193014
Bannard, 2009, Modeling children's early grammatical knowledge, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 106, 17284, 10.1073/pnas.0905638106
Baroni, 2014, Don’t count, predict! a systematic comparison of context-counting vs. context-predicting semantic vectors, 238
Barsalou, 1999, Perceptual symbol systems, Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 22, 577, 10.1017/S0140525X99002149
Barsalou, 2008, Grounded cognition, Annual Review of Psychology, 59, 617, 10.1146/annurev.psych.59.103006.093639
Baumgartner, J., Zannettou, S., Keegan, B., Squire, M., & Blackburn, J. (2020, May). The pushshift reddit dataset. In Proceedings of the International AAAI Conference on Web and Social Media (Vol. 14, pp. 830-839).
Beckner, 2009, Language is a complex adaptive system: Position paper, Language Learning, 59, 1, 10.1111/j.1467-9922.2009.00533.x
Bhatia, 2017, Associative judgment and vector space semantics, Psychological Review, 124, 1, 10.1037/rev0000047
Bhatia, 2019, Distributed semantic representations for modelling human judgment, Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences, 29, 31, 10.1016/j.cobeha.2019.01.020
Bhatia, 2018, Naturalistic multiattribute choice, Cognition, 179, 71, 10.1016/j.cognition.2018.05.025
Bock, 1986, Syntactic persistence in language production, Cognitive Psychology, 18, 355, 10.1016/0010-0285(86)90004-6
Borghi, 2014, Vol. 2
Bruni, 2014, Multimodal distributional semantics, Journal of Artificial Intelligence Research, 49, 1, 10.1613/jair.4135
Brysbaert, 2009, Moving beyond Kučera and Francis: A critical evaluation of current word frequency norms and the introduction of a new and improved word frequency measure for American English, Behavior Research Methods, 41, 977, 10.3758/BRM.41.4.977
Brysbaert, 2016, How many words do we know? Practical estimates of vocabulary size dependent on word definition, the degree of language input and the participant’s age, Frontiers in Psychology, 7, 1116, 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01116
Brysbaert, 2019, Word prevalence norms for 62,000 English lemmas, Behavior Research Methods, 51, 467, 10.3758/s13428-018-1077-9
Buchanan, 2001, Characterizing semantic space: Neighborhood effects in word recognition, Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 8, 531, 10.3758/BF03196189
Bullinaria, 2007, Extracting semantic representations from word co-occurrence statistics: A computational study, Behavior Research Methods, 39, 510, 10.3758/BF03193020
Bullinaria, 2012, Extracting semantic representations from word co-occurrence statistics: Stop-lists, stemming, and SVD, Behavior Research Methods, 44, 890, 10.3758/s13428-011-0183-8
Bruni, E., Boleda, G., Baroni, M., & Tran, N. K. (2012). Distributional semantics in technicolor. In Proceedings of the 50th Annual Meeting of the Association for Computational Linguistics: Long Papers-Volume 1 (pp. 136-145). Association for Computational Linguistics.
Caliskan, 2017, Semantics derived automatically from language corpora contain human-like biases, Science, 356, 183, 10.1126/science.aal4230
Christiansen, 2008, Language as shaped by the brain, Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 31, 489, 10.1017/S0140525X08004998
Coltheart, 2001, DRC: A dual route cascaded model of visual word recognition and reading aloud, Psychological Review, 108, 204, 10.1037/0033-295X.108.1.204
Crump, 2020, Controlling the retrieval of general versus specific semantic knowledge in the instance theory of semantic memory
De Deyne, 2021, Visual and Affective Multimodal Models of Word Meaning in Language and Mind, Cognitive Science, 45, 10.1111/cogs.12922
Finkelstein, L., Gabrilovich, E., Matias, Y., Rivlin, E., Solan, Z., Wolfman, G., & Ruppin, E. (2001). Placing search in context: The concept revisited. In Proceedings of the 10th international conference on World Wide Web (pp. 406-414). ACM.
Firth, 1957
Gärdenfors, 2018, Levels of communication and lexical semantics, Synthese, 195, 549, 10.1007/s11229-014-0493-3
Glenberg, 2000, Symbol grounding and meaning: A comparison of high-dimensional and embodied theories of meaning, Journal of Memory and Language, 43, 379, 10.1006/jmla.2000.2714
Goh, 2020, The Auditory English Lexicon Project: A multi-talker, multi-region psycholinguistic database of 10,170 spoken words and nonwords, Behavior Research Methods, 52, 2202, 10.3758/s13428-020-01352-0
Goldberg, Y., Levy, O., 2014. word2vec Explained: deriving Mikolov et al.’s negative-sampling word-embedding method. arXiv preprint arXiv:1402.3722.
Goldinger, 1998, Echoes of echoes? An episodic trace theory of lexical access, Psychological Review, 105, 251, 10.1037/0033-295X.105.2.251
Griffiths, 2007, Topics in semantic representation, Psychological Review, 114, 211, 10.1037/0033-295X.114.2.211
Günther, 2019, Vector-space models of semantic representation from a cognitive perspective: A discussion of common misconceptions, Perspectives on Psychological Science, 14, 1006, 10.1177/1745691619861372
Halawi, G., Dror, G., Gabrilovich, E., & Koren, Y. (2012). Large-scale learning of word relatedness with constraints. In Proceedings of the 18th ACM SIGKDD international conference on Knowledge discovery and Data Mining (pp. 1406-1414). ACM.
Herdağdelen, 2017, Social media and language processing: How Facebook and Twitter provide the best frequency estimates for studying word recognition, Cognitive Science, 41, 976, 10.1111/cogs.12392
Hills, 2012, Optimal foraging in semantic memory, Psychological Review, 119, 431, 10.1037/a0027373
Hintzman, 1986, “Schema abstraction” in a multiple-trace memory model, Psychological Review, 93, 411, 10.1037/0033-295X.93.4.411
Hoffman, 2013, Semantic diversity: A measure of semantic ambiguity based on variability in the contextual usage of words, Behavior Research Methods, 45, 718, 10.3758/s13428-012-0278-x
Hollis, 2017, Estimating the average need of semantic knowledge from distributional semantic models, Memory and Cognition, 45, 1350, 10.3758/s13421-017-0732-1
Hollis, 2020, Delineating linguistic contexts, and the validity of context diversity as a measure of a word’s contextual variability, Journal of Memory and Language, 114, 104146, 10.1016/j.jml.2020.104146
Hsiao, 2018, Semantic diversity, frequency and the development of lexical quality in children’s word reading, Journal of Memory and Language, 103, 114, 10.1016/j.jml.2018.08.005
Jamieson, 2018, An instance theory of semantic memory, Computational Brain & Behavior, 1, 119, 10.1007/s42113-018-0008-2
Johns, 2010, Evaluating the random representation assumption of lexical semantics in cognitive models, Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 17, 662, 10.3758/PBR.17.5.662
Johns, 2012, A synchronization account of false recognition, Cognitive Psychology, 65, 486, 10.1016/j.cogpsych.2012.07.002
Johns, 2012, Perceptual Inference through global lexical similarity, Topics in Cognitive Science, 4, 103, 10.1111/j.1756-8765.2011.01176.x
Johns, 2015, Generating structure from experience: A retrieval-based model of language processing, Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology, 69, 233, 10.1037/cep0000053
Johns, 2016, The influence of contextual diversity on word learning, Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 23, 1214, 10.3758/s13423-015-0980-7
Johns, 2021, Content matters: Measures of contextual diversity must consider semantic content, PsyArXiv.
Johns, 2018, Cognitive modeling as an interface between brain and behavior: Measuring the semantic decline in mild cognitive impairment, Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology, 72, 117, 10.1037/cep0000132
Johns, 2018, A large-scale analysis of variance in written language, Cognitive Science, 42, 1360, 10.1111/cogs.12583
Johns, 2019, The influence of time and place on lexical behavior: A distributional analysis, Behavior Research Methods, 51, 2438, 10.3758/s13428-019-01289-z
Johns, 2019, The role of negative information in distributional semantic learning, Cognitive Science, 43, e12730, 10.1111/cogs.12730
Johns, 2019, Using experiential optimization to build lexical representations, Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 26, 103, 10.3758/s13423-018-1501-2
Johns, 2021, A continuous source reinstatement model of true and false recollection, Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology, 75, 1, 10.1037/cep0000237
Johns, 2020, Estimating the prevalence and diversity of words in written language, Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 73, 841, 10.1177/1747021819897560
Johns, 2020, The continued importance of theory: Lessons from big data approaches to cognition
Johns, 2020, Production without rules: Using an instance memory model to exploit structure in natural language, Journal of Memory and Language, 115, 104165, 10.1016/j.jml.2020.104165
Johns, 2019, Mining a crowdsourced dictionary to understand consistency and preference in word meanings, Frontiers in Psychology, 10, 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.00268
Johns, 2021, Disentangling contextual diversity: Communicative need as a lexical organizer, Psychological Review, 128, 525, 10.1037/rev0000265
Johns, B. T. (in press). Accounting for item-level variance in recognition memory: Comparing word frequency and contextual diversity. Memory & Cognition.
Jones, 2007, Representing word meaning and order information in a composite holographic lexicon, Psychological Review, 114, 1, 10.1037/0033-295X.114.1.1
Jones, 2012, The role of semantic diversity in lexical organization, Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology, 66, 115, 10.1037/a0026727
Jones, M. N., Willits, J., & Dennis, S. (2014). Models of semantic memory. In J. R. Busemeyer & J. T. Townsend (Eds.) Oxford Handbook of Mathematical and Computational Psychology.
Jones, M. N., Dye, M., & Johns, B. T. (2017). Context as an organizational principle of the lexicon. In B. Ross (Ed.), The Psychology of Learning and Motivation, 67:43.
Jones, 2018, When does abstraction occur in semantic memory: Insights from distributional models, Language, Cognition and Neuroscience, 34, 1338, 10.1080/23273798.2018.1431679
Keuleers, 2012, The British Lexicon Project: Lexical decision data for 28,730 monosyllabic and disyllabic English words, Behavior Research Methods, 44, 287, 10.3758/s13428-011-0118-4
Kumar, 2020, Semantic memory: A review of methods, models, and current challenges, Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 1
Kwantes, 2005, Using context to build semantics, Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 12, 703, 10.3758/BF03196761
Landauer, 1997, A solution to Plato's problem: The latent semantic analysis theory of acquisition, induction, and representation of knowledge, Psychological Review, 104, 211, 10.1037/0033-295X.104.2.211
Lazaridou, 2017, Multimodal word meaning induction from minimal exposure to natural text, Cognitive Science, 41, 677, 10.1111/cogs.12481
Levy, 2014, Neural word embedding as implicit matrix factorization, 2177
Levy, 2015, Improving distributional similarity with lessons learned from word embedding, Transactions of the Association for Computational Linguistics, 3, 211, 10.1162/tacl_a_00134
Lieven, 1997, Lexically-based learning and early grammatical development, Journal of Child Language, 24, 187, 10.1017/S0305000996002930
Lynott, 2019, The Lancaster Sensorimotor Norms: Multidimensional measures of perceptual and action strength for 40,000 English words, Behavior Research Methods, 52, 1271, 10.3758/s13428-019-01316-z
Mak, 2021, Anchoring and contextual variation in the early stages of incidental word learning during reading, Journal of Memory and Language, 118, 104203, 10.1016/j.jml.2020.104203
Mandera, 2020, Recognition times for 62 thousand English words: Data from the English Crowdsourcing Project, Behavior Research Methods, 52, 741, 10.3758/s13428-019-01272-8
Marelli, 2015, Affixation in semantic space: Modeling morpheme meanings with compositional distributional semantics, Psychological Review, 122, 485, 10.1037/a0039267
Marelli, 2017, Compounding as Abstract Operation in Semantic Space: Investigating relational effects through a large-scale, data-driven computational model, Cognition, 166, 207, 10.1016/j.cognition.2017.05.026
McRae, 2005, Semantic feature production norms for a large set of living and nonliving things, Behavior Research Methods, 37, 547, 10.3758/BF03192726
Mewhort, 2018, Release from PI: An analysis and a model, Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 25, 932, 10.3758/s13423-017-1327-3
Mikolov, 2013, Distributed representations of words and phrases and their compositionality, 3111
Murray, 2004, Serial mechanisms in lexical access: The rank hypothesis, Psychological Review, 111, 721, 10.1037/0033-295X.111.3.721
Osth, 2020, Global semantic similarity effects in recognition memory: Insights from BEAGLE representations and the diffusion decision model, Journal of Memory and Language, 111, 104071, 10.1016/j.jml.2019.104071
van Overschelde, 2004, Category norms: An updated and expanded version of the norms, Journal of Memory and Language, 50, 289, 10.1016/j.jml.2003.10.003
Park, 2018, Examining thematic similarity, difference, and membership in three online mental health communities from Reddit: A text mining and visualization approach, Computers in Human Behavior, 78, 98, 10.1016/j.chb.2017.09.001
Pickering, 2008, Structural priming: A critical review, Psychological Bulletin, 134, 427, 10.1037/0033-2909.134.3.427
Radinsky, K., Agichtein, E., Gabrilovich, E., & Markovitch, S. (2011, March). A word at a time: Computing word relatedness using temporal semantic analysis. In Proceedings of the 20th International Conference on World Wide Web (pp. 337-346). ACM.
Recchia, 2009, More data trumps smarter algorithms: Comparing pointwise mutual information to latent semantic analysis, Behavior Research Methods, 41, 657, 10.3758/BRM.41.3.647
Recchia, 2015, Reproducing affective norms with lexical co-occurrence statistics: Predicting valence, arousal, and dominance, Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 68, 1584, 10.1080/17470218.2014.941296
Recchia, 2015, Encoding sequential information in semantic space models: Comparing holographic reduced representation and random permutation, Computational Intelligence and Neuroscience, 2015, 1, 10.1155/2015/986574
Riordan, 2011, Redundancy in perceptual and linguistic experience: Comparing feature-based and distributional models of semantic representation, Topics in Cognitive Science, 3, 303, 10.1111/j.1756-8765.2010.01111.x
Rubenstein, 1965, Contextual correlates of synonymy, Communications of the ACM, 8, 627, 10.1145/365628.365657
Schwartz, 2013, Personality, gender, and age in the language of social media: The open-vocabulary approach, PLoS ONE, 8, e73791, 10.1371/journal.pone.0073791
Shaoul, 2010, Exploring lexical co-occurrence space using HiDEx, Behavior Research Methods, 42, 393, 10.3758/BRM.42.2.393
Shaoul, 2010
Shiffrin, 2008, A survey of model evaluation approaches with a tutorial on hierarchical Bayesian methods, Cognitive Science, 32, 1248, 10.1080/03640210802414826
Shiffrin, 1997, A model for recognition memory: REM—retrieving effectively from memory, Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 4, 145, 10.3758/BF03209391
Snefjella, 2020, How emotion is learned: Semantic learning of novel words in emotional contexts, Journal of Memory and Language, 115, 104171, 10.1016/j.jml.2020.104171
Taler, 2013, A computational analysis of semantic structure in bilingual fluency, Journal of Memory and Language, 69, 607, 10.1016/j.jml.2013.08.004
Taler, 2020, A large scale semantic analysis of verbal fluency across the aging spectrum: Data from the Canadian longitudinal study on aging, Journal of Gerontology: Psychological Sciences, 75, e221, 10.1093/geronb/gbz003
Tomasello, 2003
Tomasello, 2009
Westbury, 2019, Conceptualizing syntactic categories as semantic categories: Unifying part-of-speech identification and semantics using co-occurrence vector averaging, Behavior Research Methods, 51, 1371, 10.3758/s13428-018-1118-4
Yap, 2011, Is more always better? Effects of semantic richness on lexical decision, speeded pronunciation, and semantic classification, Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 18, 742, 10.3758/s13423-011-0092-y