Contextual Cueing: Implicit Learning and Memory of Visual Context Guides Spatial Attention
Tóm tắt
Từ khóa
Tài liệu tham khảo
Ballard, 1995, Memory representations in natural tasks, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 7, 66, 10.1162/jocn.1995.7.1.66
Berry, 1993
Biederman, 1981, On the semantics of a glance at a scene
Biederman, 1982, Scene perception: Detecting and judging objects undergoing relational violations, Cognitive Psychology, 14, 143, 10.1016/0010-0285(82)90007-X
Boyce, 1989, Effect of background information on object identification, Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 15, 556
Bravo, 1992, The role of attention in different visual-search tasks, Perception and Psychophysics, 51, 465, 10.3758/BF03211642
Buchner, 1998, Differences and commonalities between implicit learning and implicit memory, 3
Cave, 1990, Modeling the role of parallel processing in visual search, Cognitive Psychology, 22, 225, 10.1016/0010-0285(90)90017-X
Cohen, 1990, Attention and structure in sequence learning, Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 16, 17
Chun, 1996, Just say no: How are visual searches terminated when there is no target present?, Cognitive Psychology, 30, 39, 10.1006/cogp.1996.0002
de Graef, 1990, Perceptual effects of scene context on object identification, Psychological Research/Psycholische Forschung, 52, 317, 10.1007/BF00868064
Desimone, 1996, Neural mechanisms for visual memory and their role in attention, Proceedings of the National Academy of Science, USA, 93, 13494, 10.1073/pnas.93.24.13494
Desimone, 1995, Neural mechanisms of selective visual attention, Annual Review of Neuroscience, 18, 193, 10.1146/annurev.ne.18.030195.001205
Duncan, 1989, Visual search and stimulus similarity, Psychological Review, 96, 433, 10.1037/0033-295X.96.3.433
Egeth, 1972, Parallel processing of multielement displays, Cognitive Psychology, 3, 674, 10.1016/0010-0285(72)90026-6
Egeth, 1984, Searching for conjunctively defined targets, Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 10, 32
Eriksen, 1985, Allocation of attention in the visual field, Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 11, 583
Friedman, 1979, Framing pictures: The role of knowledge in automatized encoding and memory for gist, Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 108, 316, 10.1037/0096-3445.108.3.316
Gibson, 1966
Grossberg, 1994, A neural theory of attentive visual search: Interactions of boundary, surface, spatial, and object representations, Psychological Review, 101, 470, 10.1037/0033-295X.101.3.470
Hintzman, 1986, “Schema abstraction” in a multiple trace memory model, Psychological Review, 93, 411, 10.1037/0033-295X.93.4.411
Humphreys, 1993, SEarch via Recursive Rejection (SERR): A connectionist model of visual search, Cognitive Psychology, 25, 43, 10.1006/cogp.1993.1002
Hunt, 1994, MacProbe: A Macintosh based experimenter's workstation for the cognitive sciences, Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers, 26, 345, 10.3758/BF03204643
Intraub, 1997, The representation of visual scenes, Trends in Cognitive Science, 1, 217, 10.1016/S1364-6613(97)01067-X
Jacoby, 1982, Remembering without awareness, Canadian Journal of Psychology, 36, 300, 10.1037/h0080638
Johnston, 1990, Attention capture by novel stimuli, Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 119, 397, 10.1037/0096-3445.119.4.397
Kinchla, 1979, Order of visual processing—top–down, bottom–up, or middle–out, Perception and Psychophysics, 25, 225, 10.3758/BF03202991
Lassaline, 1993, Memory-based automaticity in the discrimination of visual numerosity, Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 19, 561
Lewicki, 1988, Acquisition of procedural knowledge about a pattern of stimuli that cannot be articulated, Cognitive Psychology, 20, 24, 10.1016/0010-0285(88)90023-0
Loftus, 1978, Cognitive determinants of fixation location during picture viewing, Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 4, 565
Logan, 1988, Towards an instance theory of automatization, Psychological Review, 95, 492, 10.1037/0033-295X.95.4.492
Logan, 1990, Repetition priming and automaticity: Common underlying assumptions, Cognitive Psychology, 22, 1, 10.1016/0010-0285(90)90002-L
Logan, 1996, The CODE theory of visual attention: An integration of space-based and object-based attention, Psychological Review, 103, 603, 10.1037/0033-295X.103.4.603
Logan, G. D. What is learned during automatization? II. Obligatory encoding of spatial location, Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance.
Logan, 1994, What is learned during automatization? The role of attention in constructing an instance, Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 20, 1022
Luck, 1997, The capacity of visual working memory for features and conjunctions, Nature, 390, 279, 10.1038/36846
Maljkovic, 1994, Priming of popout: I. Role of features, Memory and Cognition, 22, 657, 10.3758/BF03209251
Maljkovic, 1996, Priming of pop-out: II. The role of position, Perception and Psychophysics, 58, 977, 10.3758/BF03206826
Maljkovic, V. Nakayama, K. 1997, Priming of pop-out: III. A short-term implicit memory system beneficial for rapid target selection
Mandler, 1976, Some of the thousand words a picture is worth, Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2, 529
Mayr, 1996, Spatial attention and implicit sequence learning: Evidence for independent learning of spatial and nonspatial sequences, Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 22, 350
McPeek, R. M. Maljkovic, V. Nakayama, K. 1998, Focal attention requirement of saccades revealed using a visual search task
Medin, 1978, Context theory of classification learning, Psychological Review, 85, 207, 10.1037/0033-295X.85.3.207
Miller, 1988, Components of the location probability effect in visual search tasks, Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 14, 453
Musen, 1990, Implicit and explicit memory for visual patterns, Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 16, 127
Navon, 1977, Forest before trees: The precedence of global features in visual perception, Cognitive Psychology, 9, 353, 10.1016/0010-0285(77)90012-3
Newell, 1981, Mechanisms of skill acquisition and the law of practice, 1
Nickerson, 1965, Short-term memory for complex meaningful visual configurations: A demonstration of capacity, Canadian Journal of Psychology, 19, 155, 10.1037/h0082899
Nissen, 1987, Attentional requirements of learning: Evidence from performance measures, Cognitive Psychology, 19, 1, 10.1016/0010-0285(87)90002-8
Nosofsky, 1997, An exemplar-based random walk model of speeded classification, Psychological Review, 104, 266, 10.1037/0033-295X.104.2.266
Oliva, 1997, Coarse blobs or fine edges? Evidence that information diagnosticity changes the perception of complex visual stimuli, Cognitive Psychology, 34, 72, 10.1006/cogp.1997.0667
O'Regan, 1992, Solving the “real” mysteries of visual perception: The world as an outside memory, Canadian Journal of Psychology, 46, 461, 10.1037/h0084327
Palmer, 1975, The effects of contextual scenes on the identification of objects, Memory and Cognition, 3, 519, 10.3758/BF03197524
Palmeri, 1997, Exemplar similarity and the development of automaticity, Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 23, 324
Pashler, 1988, Familiarity and visual change detection, Perception and Psychophysics, 44, 369, 10.3758/BF03210419
Posner, 1968, On the genesis of abstract ideas, Journal of Experimental Psychology, 77, 353, 10.1037/h0025953
Reber, 1989, Implicit learning and tacit knowledge, Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 118, 219, 10.1037/0096-3445.118.3.219
Rensink, 1997, To see or not to see: The need for attention to perceive changes in scenes, Psychological Science, 8, 368, 10.1111/j.1467-9280.1997.tb00427.x
Roediger, 1990, Implicit memory: Retention without remembering, American Psychologist, 45, 1043, 10.1037/0003-066X.45.9.1043
Sanocki, 1997, Priming spatial layout of scenes, Psychological Science, 8, 374, 10.1111/j.1467-9280.1997.tb00428.x
Schacter, 1994
Schacter, 1987, Implicit memory: History and current status, Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 13, 501
Schneider, 1977, Controlled and automatic human information processing: I. Detection, search and attention, Psychological Review, 84, 1, 10.1037/0033-295X.84.1.1
Schyns, 1994, From blobs to boundary edges: Evidence for time and spatial scale dependent scene recognition, Psychological Science, 5, 195, 10.1111/j.1467-9280.1994.tb00500.x
Shaw, 1978, A capacity allocation model for reaction time, Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 4, 586
Shaw, 1977, Optimal allocation of cognitive resources to spatial locations, Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, 3, 201
Shepard, 1967, Recognition memory for words, sentences, and pictures, Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 6, 156, 10.1016/S0022-5371(67)80067-7
Shiffrin, 1977, Controlled and automatic human information processing: II. Perceptual learning, automatic attending and a general theory, Psychological Review, 84, 127, 10.1037/0033-295X.84.2.127
Simons, 1996, In sight, out of mind: When object representations fail, Psychological Science, 7, 301, 10.1111/j.1467-9280.1996.tb00378.x
Sperling, 1960, The information available in brief visual presentations, Psychological Monographs: General and Applied, 74, 1, 10.1037/h0093759
Squire, 1992, Declarative and nondeclarative memory: Multiple brain systems supporting learning and memory, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 99, 195
Stadler, 1994, Whither learning, whither memory?, Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 17, 423, 10.1017/S0140525X00035342
Stadler, 1998
Standing, 1973, Learning 10,000 pictures, Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 25, 207, 10.1080/14640747308400340
Suzuki, 1993, Retinotopic vs object-centered specificity of learning differs for threshold and reaction time tasks, Investigative Ophthalmology and Visual Science, 34, S1233
Theeuwes, 1992, Perceptual selectivity for color and form, Perception and Psychophysics, 51, 599, 10.3758/BF03211656
Treisman, 1988, Features and objects: The fourteenth Bartlett memorial lecture, The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 40A, 201, 10.1080/02724988843000104
Treisman, 1988, Feature analysis in early vision: Evidence from search asymmetries, Psychological Review, 95, 15, 10.1037/0033-295X.95.1.15
Treisman, 1992, Automaticity and preattentive processing. Special issue: Views and varieties of automaticity, American Journal of Psychology, 105, 341, 10.2307/1423032
Treisman, 1980, A feature-integration theory of attention, Cognitive Psychology, 12, 97, 10.1016/0010-0285(80)90005-5
Wang, 1994, Familiarity and pop-out in visual search, Perception and Psychophysics, 56, 495, 10.3758/BF03206946
Wolfe, 1994, Guided search 2.0: A revised model of guided search, Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 1, 202, 10.3758/BF03200774
Wolfe, 1994, Visual search in continuous, naturalistic stimuli, Vision Research, 34, 1187, 10.1016/0042-6989(94)90300-X
Wolfe, 1989, Guided search: An alternative to the feature integration model for visual search, Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, 15, 419
Wolfe, J. M. Klempen, N. Dahlen, K. 1997, Post-attentive vision
Yantis, 1996, Attentional capture in vision, 45
Yantis, 1984, Abrupt visual onsets and selective attention: Evidence from visual search, Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, 10, 601