Configural association theory: The role of the hippocampal formation in learning, memory, and amnesia
Tóm tắt
Từ khóa
Tài liệu tham khảo
Ackley, D. H., Hinton, G. E., & Sejnowski, T. J. (1985). A learning algorithm for Boltzmann machines. Cognitive Science, 9, 147–169.
Aggleton, J. P., Hunt, P. R., & Rawlins, J. N. P. (1986). The effects of hippocampal lesions upon spatial and non-spatial tests of working memory. Behavioural Brain Research, 19, 133–146.
Aggleton, J. P., & Mishkin, M. (1985). The amygdala: Sensory gateway to emotions. In R. Plutchik & H. Kellerman (Eds.), Emotion: Theory, research, and experience (Vol. 3, pp. 281–299). New York: Academic Press.
Amsel, A. (1958). The role of frustrative nonreward in noncontinuous reward situations. Psychological Bulletin, 55, 102–119.
Amsel, A. (1962). Frustrative nonreward in partial reinforcement and discrimination learning. Psychological Review, 69, 306–328.
Bachevauer, J., Saunders, R. C, & Mishkin, M. (1985). Visual recognition in monkeys: Effects of transection of fornix. Experimental Brain Research, 57, 547–553.
Bagshaw, M. H., & Benzies, S. (1968). Multiple measures of the orienting reaction and their dissociation after amygdalectomy in monkeys. Experimental Neurology, 20, 175–187.
Bagshaw, M. H., & Coppock, H. W. (1968). Galvanic skin response conditioning deficit in amygdalectomized monkeys. Experimental Neurology, 20, 188–196.
Barnes, C. A. (1988). Spatial learning and memory processes: The search for their neurobiological mechanisms in the rat. Trends in Neurosciences, 11, 163–169.
Barto, A. G. (1985). Learning by statistical cooperation of self-interested neuron-like computing elements (COINS Technical Report No. 85-11). Amherst, MA: University of Massachusetts, Department of Computer and Information Science.
Bellingham, W. P., Gillette-Belungham, K., & Kehoe, E. J. (1985). Summation and configuration in patterning schedules with the rat and rabbit. Animal Learning & Behavior, 13, 152–164.
Berger, T. W., & Orr, W. B. (1983). Hippocampectomy selectively disrupts discrimination reversal learning of the rabbit nictitating membrane response. Behavioural Brain Research, 8, 49–68.
Best, P. J., & Orr, J., Jr. (1973). Effects of hippocampal lesions on passive avoidance and taste aversion conditioning. Physiology & Behavior, 10, 193–196.
Cohen, N. J., & Squire, L. R. (1980). Preserved learning and retention of pattern analysing skill in amnesia: Dissociation of knowing how and knowing that. Science, 210, 207–209.
Eichenbaum, H., Fagan, A., & Cohen, N. J. (1986). Normal olfactory discrimination learning set and facilitation of reversal learning after medial-temporal damage in rats: Implications for an account of preserved learning abilities in amnesia. Journal of Neuroscience, 6, 1876–1884.
Gaffan, D. (1974). Recognition impaired and association intact in the memory of monkeys after transection of the fornix. Journal of Comparative & Physiological Psychology, 86, 1100–1109.
Gaffan, D., & Harrison, S. (1984). Reversal learning by fornix transected monkeys. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 36, 223–234.
Gaffan, D., Saunders, R. C., Gaffan, E. A., Harrison, S., Shields, C., & Owen, M. J. (1984). Effects of fornix transection upon associative memory in monkeys: Role of the hippocampus in learned action. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 36, 173–221.
Gage, P. D. (1985). Preserved and impaired information processing systems in human bilateral amnesiacs and infrahuman analogues: Role of hippocampectomy. Journal of Mind & Behavior, 4, 515–552.
Grossberg, S. (1982). Studies of mind and brain: Neural principles of learning, perception, development, cognition, and motor control. Boston: Reidel Press.
Hirsh, R. (1974). The hippocampus and contextual retrieval of information from memory: A theory. Behavioral Biology, 12, 421–444.
Hirsh, R. (1980). The hippocampus, conditional operations and cognition. Physiological Psychology, 8, 175–182.
Hirsh, R., Leber, B., & Gillman, K. (1978). Fornix fibers and motivational states as controllers of behavior: A study stimulated by the contextual retrieval theory. Behavioral Biology, 22, 463–478.
Hsiao, S., & Isaacson, R. L. (1971). Learning of food and water positions by hippocampus damaged rats. Physiology & Behavior, 6, 81–83.
Hull, C. L. (1942). Principles of behavior. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.
Jarrard, L. E. (1983). Selective hippocampal lesions and behavior: Effects of kainic acid lesions on performance of place and cue tasks. Behavioral Neuroscience, 97, 873–889.
Jarrard, L. E. (1986). Selective hippocampal lesions and behavior: Implications for current research and theorizing. In R. L. Isaacson & K. H. Pribram (Eds.), The hippocampus (Vol. 4, pp. 93–127). New York: Plenum.
Jarrard, L. E., Okaichi, H., Goldschmidt, R., & Stewart, O. (1984). On the role of the hippocampal connections in the performance of place and cue tasks: Comparison with damage to the hippocampus. Behavioral Neuroscience, 98, 946–954.
Jones, B., & Mishkin, M. (1972). Limbic lesions and the problem of stimulus-reinforcement associations. Experimental Neurology, 36, 362–377.
Kamin, L. J. (1969). Predictability, surprise, attention, and conditioning. In B. A. Campbell & R. M. Church (Eds.), Punishment and aver-sive behavior. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.
Kaye, H., & Pearce, J. M. (1987). Hippocampal lesions attenuate latent inhibition and the decline of the orienting response in rats. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 39B, 107–125.
Kaye, H., Preston, G. C, Szabo, L., Druiff, H., & Mackintosh, N. J. (1987). Context specificity of conditioning and latent inhibition: Evidence for a dissociation of latent inhibition and associative interference. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 39B, 127–145.
Kehoe, E. J. (1988). A layered network model of associative learning: Learning-to-learn and configuration. Psychological Review, 95, 411–433.
Kimble, D. P. (1963). The effects of bilateral hippocampal lesions in rats. Journal of Comparative & Physiological Psychology, 56, 273–283.
Mackintosh, N. J. (1975). A theory of selective attention: Variation in the associability of stimuli with reinforcement. Psychological Review, 82, 276–298.
Mahut, H. (1971). Spatial and object reversal learning in monkeys with partial temporal lobe ablations. Neuropsychologia, 9, 409–424.
Mahut, H., & Cordeau, J. P. (1963). Spatial reversal deficit in monkeys after amygdalohippocampal ablations. Experimental Neurology, 7, 426–434.
Mahut, H., & Moss, M. (1986). The monkey and the sea horse. In R. L. Isaacson & K. H. Pribram (Eds.), The Hippocampus: (Vol. 4, pp. 241–279). New York: Plenum.
Mahut, H., Moss, M., & Zola-Morgan, S. (1981). Retention deficits after combined amygdala-hippocampal and selective lesions in the monkey. Neuropsychologia, 19, 201–225.
Mahut, H., Zola-Morgan, S., & Moss, M. (1982). Hippocampal lesions impair associative learning and memory in the monkey. Journal of Neuroscience, 2, 1214–1229.
McNaughton, B. L. (1989). Neuronal mechanisms for spatial computation and information storage. In L. Nadel, L. Cooper, P. Culicover, & R. M. Harnish (Eds.), Neural connections and mental computations (pp. 285–350). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press/Bradford Books.
Milner, B. (1959). The memory defect in bilateral hippocampal lesions. Psychiatric Research Reports, 11, 43–52.
Milner, B. (1962). Les troubles de la memoire accompagnant des lesions hippocampiques bilaterales. In P. Passouant (Ed.), Physiologie de l’hippocampe (pp. 257–272). Paris: Centre National de la Recherche Scientifique.
Milner, B. (1970). Memory and the medial temporal regions of the brain. In K. H. Pribram & D. E. Broadbent (Eds.), Biology of memory (pp. 29–50). New York: Academic Press.
Mishkin, M. (1978). Memory in monkeys severely impaired by combined but not by separate removal of amygdala or hippocampus. Nature, 273, 297–298.
Mishkin, M., Malamut, B., & Bachevalier, J. (1984). Memories and habits: Two neural systems. In G. Lynch, J. L. McGaugh, & N. M. Weinberger (Eds.), Neurobiology of learning and memory (pp. 65–88). New York: Guilford Press.
Mishkin, M., & Petri, H. L. (1984). Memories and habits: Some implications for the analysis of learning and retention. In L. R. Squire & N. Butters (Eds.), Neuropsychology of memory (pp. 287–296). New York: Guilford Press.
Morris, R. G. M. (1981). Spatial localization does not require the presence of local cues. Learning & Motivation, 12, 239–260.
Morris, R. G. M., Garrud, P., Rawlins, J. N. P., & O’Keefe, J. (1982). Place navigation impaired in rats with hippocampal lesions. Nature, 297, 681–683.
Moss, M., Mahut, H., & Zola-Morgan, S. (1981). Concurrent discrimination learning of monkeys after hippocampal, entorhinal, or fornix lesions. Journal of Neuroscience, 1, 222–240.
Murray, E. A., & Mishkin, M. (1984). Severe tactual as well as visual memory deficits following combined removal of the amygdala and hippocampus in monkeys. Journal ofNeuroscience, 4, 2565–2580.
Nachman, M., & Ashe, J. H. (1974). Effects of bilateral amygdala lesions on neophobia, learned taste aversions, and sodium appetite in rats. Journal of Comparative & Physiological Psychology, 80, 622–643.
Nadel, L., & MacDonald, L. (1980). Hippocampus: Cognitive map or working memory? Behavioral & Neural Biology, 29, 405–409.
Nadel, L., & O’Keefe, J. (1974). The hippocampus in pieces and patches: An essay on modes of explanation in physiological psychology. In R. Bellairs & J. Gray (Eds.), Essays on the nervous system: A festschrift for Professor J. Z Young (pp. 367–390). Oxford, England: Clarendon Press.
Nadel, L., & Willner, J. (1980). Context and conditioning: A place for space. Physiological Psychology, 8, 218–228.
Nadel, L., Willner, J., & Kurz, E. (1985). Cognitive maps and environment context. In P. D. Balsam & A. Tomie (Eds.), Context and learning (pp. 385–406). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Odling-Smee, F. J. (1975). The role of background stimuli during Pav-lovian conditioning. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 27, 201–209.
Okaichi, H. (1987). Performance and dominant strategies on place and cue tasks following hippocampal lesions in rats. Psychobiology, 15, 58–63.
O’Keefe, J., & Nadel, L. (1978). The hippocampus as a cognitive map. Oxford, England: Clarendon Press.
O’Keefe, J., & Nadel, L. (1979). Precis of O’Keefe & Nadel’s “The hippocampus as a cognitive map.” Brain & Behavioral Sciences, 2, 487–533.
O’Keefe, J., Nadel, L., Keightly, S., & Kill, D. (1975). Fornix lesions selectively abolish place learning in the rat. Experimental Neurology, 48, 152–166.
Olton, D. S., Becker, J. T., & Handelmann, G. E. (1979). Hippocampus, space and memory. Behavioral & Brain Sciences, 2, 313–365.
Olton, D. S., & Feustle, W. (1981). Hippocampal function and non-spatial memory. Experimental Brain Research, 411, 380–389.
Olton, D. S., & Papas, B. C. (1979). Spatial memory and hippocampal function. Neuropsychologia, 17, 669–682.
Olton, D. S., & Samuelson, R. J. (1976). Remembrance of places passed: Spatial memory in rats. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 2, 97–116.
Parkinson, J. K., & Mishkin, M. (1982). A selective role for the hippocampus in monkeys: Memory for the location of objects. Society of Neuroscience Abstracts, 23.
Parkinson, J. K., Murray, E. A., & Mishkin, M. (1988). A selective mnemonic role for the hippocampus in monkeys: Memory for the location of objects. Journal of Neuroscience, 8, 4159–4167.
Raffaele, K. C, & Olton, D. S. (1988). Hippocampal and amygdala involvement in working memory for nonspatial stimuli. Behavioral Neuroscience, 102, 349–355.
Rawlins, J. N. P. (1985). Associations across time: The hippocampus as a temporary memory store. Behavioral & Brain Sciences, 8, 479–96.
Rescorla, R. A. (1972). “Configural” conditioning in discrete-trial bar pressing. Journal of Comparative & Physiological Psychology, 79 307–317.
Rescorla, R. A., & Wagner, A. R. (1972). A theory of Pavlovian conditioning: Variations in the effectiveness of reinforcement and non-reinforcement. In A. H. Black & W. F. Prokasy (Eds.), Classical conditioning: II. Current research and theory (pp. 64–99). New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.
Restle, F. (1957). Discrimination of cues in mazes: A resolution of the “place-vs-response” question. Psychological Review, 64, 217–228.
Rickert, E. J., Bennett, T. L., Lane, P., & French, J. (1978). Hippo-campectomy and the attenuation of blocking. Behavioral Biology, 22, 147–160.
Rickert, E. J., Lorden, J. F., Dawson, R., Jr., Smyly, E., & Callahan, M. F. (1979). Stimulus processing and stimulus selection in rats with hippocampal lesions. Behavioral & Neural Biology, 27, 454–465.
Rolls, E. T., & Rolls, B. J. (1973). Altered food preferences after lesions in the basolateral region of the amygdala in the rat. Journal of Comparative & Physiological Psychology, 83, 248–259.
Ross, R. T., Orr, W. B., Holland, P. C, & Berger, T. W. (1984). Hippocampectomy disrupts acquisition and retention of learned conditional responding. Behavioral Neuroscience, 98, 211–225.
Rumelhart, D. E., Hinton, G. E., & Wiluams, R. J. (1986). Learning internal representations by error propagation. In D. E. Rumelhart & J. L. McClelland (Eds.), Parallel distributed processing: Explorations in the microstructure of cognition: Vol. I. Foundations (pp. 318–362). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Salmon, D., Zola-Mnr]organ, S., & Squire, L. R. (1987). Retrograde amnesia following combined hippocampal-amygdala lesions in monkeys. Psychobiology, 15, 37–47.
Saunders, R. C, Murray, E. A., & Mishkin, M. (1984). Further evidence that amygdala and hippocampus contribute equally to visual recognition. Neuropsychologia, 22, 785–796.
Scoville, W. B., & Milner, B. (1957). Loss of recent memory after bilateral hippocampal lesions. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery & Psychiatry, 20, 11–21.
Solomon, P. R. (1977). Role of hippocampus in blocking and conditioned inhibition of the rabbit’s nictitating membrane response. Journal of Comparative & Physiological Psychology, 91, 407–417.
Solomon, P. R., & Moore, J. W. (1975). Latent inhibition and stimulus generalization of the classically conditioned nictitating membrane response in rabbits (Oryctolagus cuniculus) following dorsal hippocampal ablation. Journal of Comparative & Physiological Psychology, 89, 1192–1203.
Squire, L. R. (1987). Memory and brain. New York: Oxford University Press.
Squire, L. R., & Cohen, N. J. (1984). Human memory and amnesia. In G. Lynch, J. L. McGaugh, & N. M. Weinberger (Eds.), Neurobiology of learning and memory (pp. 3–64). New York: Guilford Press.
Squire, L. R., Cohen, N. J., & Nadel, L. (1984). The medial temporal region and memory consolidation: A new hypothesis. In H. Weingartner & W. Parker (Eds.), Memory consolidation (pp. 185–210). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
SQUIRE, L. R., SHIMAMURA, A. P., & AMARAL, D. G. (in press). Memory and the hippocampus. In J. Byrne & W. Berry (Eds.), Neural models of plasticity. New York: Academic Press.
Squire, L. R., & Zola-Morgan, S. (1983). The neurology of memory: The case for correspondence between the findings for human and non-human primates. In J. A. Deutsch (Ed.), The physiological basis of memory (2nd ed., pp. 199–267). New York: Academic Press.
Sutherland, R. J. (1985). The navigating hippocampus: An individual medley of space, memory and movement. In G. Buzsaki & C. H. Vanderwolf (Eds.), Electrical activity of the archicortex (pp. 255–279). Budapest: Akadémiai Kiadó.
Sutherland, R. J., Arnold, K. A., & Rodriguez, A. R. (1987). Anterograde and retrograde effects on place memory after limbic or diencephalic damage. Society of Neuroscience Abstracts, 13, 1066.
Sutherland, R. J., Kolb, B., & Whishaw, I. Q. (1982). Spatial mapping: Definitive disruption by hippocampal or medial frontal cortical damage in the rat. Neuroscience Letters, 31, 271–276.
Sutherland, R. J., Whishaw, I. Q., & Kolb, B. (1983). A behavioural analysis of spatial localization following electrolytic, kainate-, or colchicine-induced damage to the hippocampal formation in the rat. Behavioural Brain Research, 1, 133–153.
Teitelbaum, H. A. (1964). A comparison of orbito-frontal and hippocampal lesions upon discrimination learning and reversal in the cat. Experimental Neurology, 90, 452–462.
Teyler, T. J., & DiScenna, P. (1986). The hippocampal memory indexing theory. Behavioral Neuroscience, 100, 147.
Thorndike, E. L. (1898). Animal intelligence: An experimental study of the associative processes in animals. Psychological Monographs, 2 (Whole No. 8).
Wagner, A. R., Logan, F. A., Haberlandt, K., & Price, T. (1968). Stimulus selection in animal discrimination learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 76, 171–180.
Whitlow, J. W., Jr., & Wagner, A. R. (1972). Negative patterning in classical conditioning: Summation of response tendencies to iso-lable and configural components. Psychonomic Science, 27, 299–301.
Wickelgren, W. A. (1979). Chunking and consolidation: A theoretical synthesis of semantic networks, configuring, S-R versus cognitive learning, normal forgetting, the amnesic syndrome, and the hippocampal arousal system. Psychological Review, 86, 44–60.
Wilson, F. A. W., & Rolls, E. T. (1987). Habituation and stimulus-specific neuronal responses recorded from the amygdala of the monkey. Society for Neuroscience Abstracts, 13, 1101.
Winocur, G., Rawlins, J. N. P., & Gray, J. A. (1987). The hippocampus and conditioning to contextual cues. Behavioral Neuroscience, 101, 617–627.
Woodbury, C. B. (1943). The learning of stimulus patterns by dogs. Journal of Comparative & Physiological Psychology, 35, 20–40.
Zola-Morgan, S., & Squire, L. R. (1986). Memory impairment in monkeys following lesions limited to the hippocampus. Behavioral Neuroscience, 100, 155–160.
Zola-Morgan, S., Squire, L. R., & Amaral, D. (1986). Human amnesia and the medial temporal region: Enduring memory impairment following a bilateral lesion limited to the CA1 field of the hippocampus. Journal of Neuroscience, 6, 2950–2967.