Cognitive individualism and the child as scientist program
Studies in History and Philosophy of Science Part C: Studies in History and Philosophy of Biological and Biomedical Sciences - Tập 42 - Trang 518-529 - 2011
Tài liệu tham khảo
Bishop, 2002, The theory thrice over: The child as scientist, superscientist or social institution?, Studies in History and Philosophy of Science, 33, 117, 10.1016/S0039-3681(01)00029-2
Downes, 1993, Socializing naturalized philosophy of science, Philosophy of Science, 60, 452, 10.1086/289746
Downes, 1999, Ontogeny, phylogeny and scientific development, 273
Downes, 2001, Agents and norms in the new economics of science, Philosophy of the Social Sciences, 31, 224, 10.1177/004839310103100204
Faucher, 2002, The baby in the lab-coat: Why child development is not an adequate model for understanding the development of science
Gauvain, 1998, Cognitive development in social and cultural context, Current Directions in Psychological Science, 7, 188, 10.1111/1467-8721.ep10836917
Gauvain, 2001, Cultural tools, social interactions, and the development of thinking, Human Development, 44, 126, 10.1159/000057052
Giere, 1988
Giere, 2002, Scientific cognition as distributed cognition
Glennan, 2005, The modeller in the crib, Philosophical Explorations, 8, 217, 10.1080/13869790500219604
Goldman, 1999
Goldman, 2004
Gopnik, 1996, The scientist as child, Philosophy of Science, 63, 485, 10.1086/289970
Gopnik, 1998, Explanation as orgasm, Minds and Machines, 8, 101, 10.1023/A:1008290415597
Gopnik, 2003, The theory as an alternative to the innateness hypothesis
Gopnik, 1997
Gopnik, 1998, Theories vs. modules: To the max and beyond a reply to Poulin-Dubois and to Stich and Nichols, Mind and Language, 13, 3, 10.1111/1468-0017.00086
Gopnik, 1999
Gopnik, 2004, Mechanisms of theory formation in young children, Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 8, 371, 10.1016/j.tics.2004.06.005
2007
Gopnik, 2007, Bayesian networks, Bayesian learning and cognitive development, Developmental Science, 10, 281, 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2007.00584.x
Hankinson Nelson, 1990
Harding, 1986
Hull, 1988
Hutchins, 1995
Kitcher, 1988, The child as parent of the scientist, Mind and Language, 3, 217, 10.1111/j.1468-0017.1988.tb00144.x
Kitcher, 1993
Knorr-Cetina, 1999
Logino, 2002
Murphy, 2006, On Fodor’s analogy: Why psychology is like philosophy of science after all, Mind and Language, 21, 553, 10.1111/j.1468-0017.2006.00291.x
Rogoff, 1997, Shared thinking: Community and institutional variations, Discourse Processes, 23, 471, 10.1080/01638539709545000
Schwitzgebel, 1999, Children’s theories and the drive to explain, Science & Education, 8, 457, 10.1023/A:1008633709447
Solomon, 1994, Social empiricism, Nous, 28, 323, 10.2307/2216062
Solomon, 1996, Commentary on Alison Gopnik’s ‘The Scientist As Child, Philosophy of Science, 63, 4, 10.1086/289975
Solomon, 2001
Staley, 2005
Sterelny, 2003
Stich, 1990
Stich, 1998, Theory to the Max, Mind and Language, 13, 421, 10.1111/1468-0017.00085
Strevens, 2003, The role of the priority rule in science, Journal of Philosophy, 100, 55, 10.5840/jphil2003100224
Thagard, 2000
van Fraassen, 1980
van Fraassen, 2002
Wray, 2000, Invisible hands and the success of science, Philosophy of Science, 67, 163, 10.1086/392767