Co-thought gesturing supports more complex problem solving in subjects with lower visual working-memory capacity
Tóm tắt
During silent problem solving, hand gestures arise that have no communicative intent. The role of such co-thought gestures in cognition has been understudied in cognitive research as compared to co-speech gestures. We investigated whether gesticulation during silent problem solving supported subsequent performance in a Tower of Hanoi problem-solving task, in relation to visual working-memory capacity and task complexity. Seventy-six participants were assigned to either an instructed gesture condition or a condition that allowed them to gesture, but without explicit instructions to do so. This resulted in three gesture groups: (1) non-gesturing; (2) spontaneous gesturing; (3) instructed gesturing. In line with the embedded/extended cognition perspective on gesture, gesturing benefited complex problem-solving performance for participants with a lower visual working-memory capacity, but not for participants with a lower spatial working-memory capacity.
Tài liệu tham khảo
Alibali, M. W., Spencer, R. C., Knox, L., & Kita, S. (2011). Spontaneous gestures influence strategy choices in problem solving. Psychological Science, 22(9), 1138–1144. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797611417722.
Beilock, S. L., & Goldin-Meadow, S. (2010). Gesture changes thought by grounding it in action. Psychological Science, 21(11), 1605–1610. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797610385353.
Brooks, N. B. (2014). Movements for doing, thinking, and speaking: The relationship between motor experience and gesture in novice mental abacus users (Doctoral dissertation, the University of Chicago).
Cappuccio, M. L., Chu, M., & Kita, S. (2013). Pointing as an instrumental gesture: Gaze representation through indication. Humana.Mente: Journal of Philosophical Studies, 24, 125–149.
Chu, M., & Kita, S. (2008). Spontaneous gestures during mental rotation tasks: Insights into the microdevelopment of the motor strategy. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 137(4), 706–723. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0013157.
Chu, M., & Kita, S. (2011). The nature of gestures’ beneficial role in spatial problem solving. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 140(1), 102–115. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0021790.
Chu, M., & Kita, S. (2016). Co-thought and co-speech gestures are generated by the same action generation process. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 42, 257–270. https://doi.org/10.1037/xlm0000168.
Chu, M., Meyer, A., Foulkes, L., & Kita, S. (2013). Individual differences in frequency and salience of speech-accompanying gestures: the role of cognitive abilities and empathy. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 143, 694–709. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0033861.
Cook, S. W., & Tatenhaus, M. K. (2009). Embodied communication: Speakers’ gestures affect listeners’ actions. Cognition, 113(1), 98–104. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2009.06.006.
Cook, S. W., Yip, T. K., & Goldin-Meadow, S. (2012). Gestures, but not meaningless movements, lighten working memory load when explaining math. Language and Cognitive Processes, 27(4), 594–610. https://doi.org/10.1080/01690965.2011.567074.
Delgado, B., Gómez, J. C., & Sarriá, E. (2011). Pointing gestures as a cognitive tool in young children: experimental evidence. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 110, 299–312. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2011.04.010.
Della Sala, S., Gray, C., Baddeley, A., & Wilson, L. (1997). The visual patterns test: A new test of short-term visual recall. Bury St. Edmunds: Thames Valley Test.
Frank, M. C., & Barner, D. (2012). Representing exact number visually using mental abacus. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 141(1), 134–149. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0024427.
Garber, P., & Goldin-Meadow, S. (2002). Gesture offers insight into problem-solving in adults and children. Cognitive Science, 26(6), 817–831. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15516709cog2606_5.
Goldin-Meadow, S., Nusbaum, H., Kelly, S. D., & Wagner, S. (2001). Explaining math: Gesturing lightens the load. Psychological Science, 12(6), 516–522. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-9280.00395.
Handley, S. J., Capon, A., Copp, C., & Harper, C. (2002). Conditional reasoning and the Tower of Hanoi: The role of spatial and verbal working memory. British Journal of Psychology, 93, 501–518.
Hayes, A. F. (2013). Introduction to mediation, moderation, and conditional process analysis: A regression-based approach. (New York: Guilford Press).
Hostetter, A. B., & Alibali, M. W. (2008). Visible embodiment: Gestures as simulated action. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 15(3), 495–514. https://doi.org/10.3758/PBR.15.3.495.
Hugdahl, K., Thomsen, T., & Ersland, L. (2006). Sex differences in visuo-spatial processing: an fMRI study of mental rotation. Neuropsychologia, 44(9), 1575–1583. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2006.01.026.
Kirsh, D. (1995). Complementary strategies: Why we use our hands when we think. In Proceedings of the seventeenth annual conference of the cognitive science society (pp. 212–217).
Kita, S., Alibali, M. W., & Chu, M. (2017). How do gestures influence thinking and speaking? The gesture-for-conceptualization hypothesis. Psychological Review, 124(3), 245.
Logan, T., Lowrie, T., & Diezmann, C. M. (2014). Co-thought gestures: Supporting students to successfully navigate map tasks. Educational Studies in Mathematics, 87, 87–102. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10649-014-9546-2.
Macken, L., & Ginns, P. (2014). Pointing and tracing gestures may enhance anatomy and physiology learning. Medical teacher, 36(7), 596–601.
Marstaller, L., & Burianová, H. (2013). Individual differences in the gesture effect on working memory. Psychonomic Bulletin Review, 20, 496–500. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-012-0365-0..
Paas, F., Tuovinen, J. E., Tabbers, H., & Van Gerven, P. W. (2003). Cognitive load measurement as a means to advance cognitive load theory. Educational Psychologist, 38(1), 63–71. https://doi.org/10.1207/S15326985EP3801_82014.00359.
Pouw, W., & Hostetter, A. (2016). Gesture as predictive action. Reti, Saperi, Linguaggi: Italian Journal of Cognitive Sciences, 3, 57–80. https://doi.org/10.12832/83918.
Pouw, W. T. J. L., Van Gog, T., & Paas, F. (2014). An embedded and embodied cognition review of instructional manipulatives. Educational Psychology Review, 26(1), 51–72. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10648-014-9255-5.
Pouw, W. T. J. L., De Nooijer, J. A., Van Gog, T., Zwaan, R. A., & Paas, F. (2014). Toward a more embedded/extended perspective on the cognitive function of gestures. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 359. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00359.
Pouw, W. T. J. L., Mavilidi, M., Van Gog, T., & Paas, F. (2016). Gesturing during mental problem solving reduces eye movements, especially for individuals with lower visual working-memory capacity. Cognitive Processing, 17(3), 269–277. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10339-016-0757-6.
Pouw, W., Van Gog, T., Zwaan, R. A., Agostinho, S., & Paas, F. (2018). Co-thought gestures in children's mental problem solving: Prevalence and effects on subsequent performance. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 32(1), 66–80.
Shephard, S., & Metzler, D. (1988). Mental rotation: effects of dimensionality of object and type of task. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 14(1), 3–11.
Smithson, L., & Nicoladis, E. (2014). Lending a hand to imagery? The impact of visuospatial working memory interference upon iconic gesture production in a narrative task. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 38, 247–258. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10919-014-0176-2.
So, W. C., Ching, T. H. W., Lim, P. E., Cheng, X., & Ip, K. Y. (2014). Producing gestures facilitates route learning. PloS one, 9(11), 1–21. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0112543.
Stephen, D. G., Dixon, J. A., & Isenhower, R. W. (2009). Dynamics of representational change: entropy, action, and cognition. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 35, 1811–1832. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0014510.
Trofatter, C., Kontra, C., Beilock, S., & Goldin-Meadow, S. (2014). Gesturing has a larger impact on problem-solving than action, even when action is accompanied by words. Language, Cognition and Neuroscience, 30(3), 251–260. https://doi.org/10.1080/23273798.2014.905692.
Wagner, S. M., Nusbaum, H., & Goldin-Meadow, S. (2004). Probing the mental representation of gesture: Is handwaving spatial? Journal of Memory and Language, 50(4), 395–407.
Zook, N. A., Davalos, D. B., Delosh, E. L., & Davis, H. P. (2004). Working memory, inhibition, and fluid intelligence as predictors of performance on Tower of Hanoi and London tasks. Brain and Cognition, 56(3), 286–292. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bandc.2004.07.003.
Zurina, H., & Williams, J. (2011). Gesturing for oneself. Educational Studies in Mathematics Education, 77(2–3), 175–188. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10649-010-9294-x.