A dual-process perspective on over-imitation
Tài liệu tham khảo
Bandura, A. (1986). Social foundations of thought and action. Prentice Hall. doi: 10.4135/9781446221129.n6.
Bandura, 1999, 154
Bandura, 2001, Social cognitive theory: An agentic perspective, Annual Review of Psychology, 52, 1, 10.1146/annurev.psych.52.1.1
Bandura, 1971
Bekkering, 2000, Imitation of gestures in children is goal-directed, The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 53A, 153, 10.1080/713755872
Beran, 2017, To Err Is (Not Only) Human: Fallibility as a window into primate cognition, Comparative Cognition & Behavior Reviews, 12, 57, 10.3819/CCBR.2017.120006
Boyer, 2006, Precaution systems and ritualized behavior, Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 29, 635, 10.1017/S0140525X06009575
Brainerd, 2002, Fuzzy-trace theory and false memory, Current Directions in Psychological Science, 11, 164, 10.1111/1467-8721.00192
Brainerd, 2002, Are young children susceptible to the False-Memory Illusion?, Child Development, 73, 1363, 10.1111/1467-8624.00477
Brosnan, 2016, Reasoning on the Autism Spectrum: A dual process theory account, Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 46, 2115, 10.1007/s10803-016-2742-4
Bulbulia, 2011, Signalling theory and the evolution of religious cooperation, Religion, 41, 363, 10.1080/0048721X.2011.604508
Buttelmann, 2013, Selective imitation of in-group over out-group members in 14-month-old infants, Child Development, 84, 422, 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2012.01860.x
Brugger, 2007, Doing the right thing: Infants’ selection of actions to imitate from observed event sequences, Child Development, 78, 806, 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2007.01034.x
Call, 2006, Descartes' two errors: Reason and reflection in the great apes, Rational Animals?, 219, 10.1093/acprof:oso/9780198528272.003.0010
Carpenter, 1998, Fourteen- through 18-month-old infants differentially imitate intentional and accidental actions, Infant Behavior and Development, 21, 315, 10.1016/S0163-6383(98)90009-1
Chartrand, 1999, The Chameleon Effect: The perception-behavior link and social interaction, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 76, 893, 10.1037/0022-3514.76.6.893
Chartrand, 2009, Human mimicry, Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 41, 219, 10.1016/S0065-2601(08)00405-X
Clay, 2017, Is overimitation a uniquely human phenomenon? Insights from human children as compared to bonobos, Child Development
Clay, 2018, What drives young children to over-imitate? Investigating the effects of age, context, action type, and transitivity, Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 166, 520, 10.1016/j.jecp.2017.09.008
Dakin, 2005, Vagaries of visual perception in autism, Neuron, 48, 497, 10.1016/j.neuron.2005.10.018
De Neys, 2006, Dual processing in reasoning, Psychological Science, 17, 428, 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2006.01723.x
DiYanni, 2008, Using a bad tool with good intention: Young children’s imitation of adults’ questionable choices, Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 101, 241, 10.1016/j.jecp.2008.05.002
Eilam, 2006, Rituals, stereotypy and compulsive behavior in animals and humans, Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 30, 456, 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2005.08.003
Etten, 2015, Does impaired social motivation drive imitation deficits in children with autism spectrum disorder?, Review Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 2, 310, 10.1007/s40489-015-0054-9
Evans, 2017, Selective copying of the majority suggests children are broadly “optimal-” rather than ‘over-’ imitators, Developmental Science, 70, e12637
Evans, 2010, Intuition and reasoning: A dual-process perspective, Psychological Inquiry, 21, 313, 10.1080/1047840X.2010.521057
Evans, 2005, Rapid responding increases belief bias: Evidence for the dual-process theory of reasoning, Thinking & Reasoning, 11, 382, 10.1080/13546780542000005
Evans, 2013, Dual-Process theories of higher cognition, Perspectives on Psychological Science, 8, 223, 10.1177/1745691612460685
Flynn, 2012, Investigating the mechanisms of cultural acquisition - How pervasive is overimitation in adults?, Social Psychology, 43, 185, 10.1027/1864-9335/a000119
Frankish, 2010, Dual-process and dual-system theories of reasoning, Philosophy Compass, 5, 914, 10.1111/j.1747-9991.2010.00330.x
Gergely, 2003, Teleological reasoning in infancy: the naïve theory of rational action, Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7, 287, 10.1016/S1364-6613(03)00128-1
Gergely, 2002, Rational imitation in preverbal infants, Nature, 415, 755, 10.1038/415755a
Gonsiorowski, 2016, Brief report: Imitation of object-directed acts in young children with autism spectrum disorders, Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 46, 691, 10.1007/s10803-015-2596-1
Gopnik, 2015, When younger learners can be better (or at least more open-minded) than older ones, Current Directions in Psychological Science, 24, 87, 10.1177/0963721414556653
Hamilton, A. F. (n.d.). The neurocognitive mechanisms of imitation. Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences, 3, 63–67. doi: 10.1016/j.cobeha.2015.01.011.
Happé, 2006, The Weak Coherence Account: Detail-focused cognitive style in Autism Spectrum Disorders, Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 36, 5, 10.1007/s10803-005-0039-0
Heyes, 2011, Automatic imitation, Psychological Bulletin, 137, 463, 10.1037/a0022288
Hilbrink, 2013, Selective and faithful imitation at 12 and 15 months, Developmental Science, 16, 828
Hoehl, 2019, Over-imitation - A review and appraisal of a decade of research, Developmental Review, 51, 90, 10.1016/j.dr.2018.12.002
Hoehl, 2014, The role of social interaction and pedagogical cues for eliciting and reducing overimitation in preschoolers, Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 122, 122, 10.1016/j.jecp.2013.12.012
Horner, 2005, Causal knowledge and imitation/emulation switching in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) and children (Homo sapiens), Animal Cognition, 8, 164, 10.1007/s10071-004-0239-6
Howard, 2015, Infants’ and young children's imitation of linguistic in-group and out-group informants, Child Development, 86, 259, 10.1111/cdev.12299
Johnson, 2010, Interactive Specialization: A domain-general framework for human functional brain development?, Accident Analysis and Prevention, 1, 7
Kahneman, 2011, Thinking, fast and slow, Farrar, Straus and Giroux.
Kahneman, 1990, Experimental tests of the endowment effect and the Coase theorem, Journal of Political Economy, 98, 1325, 10.1086/261737
Kapitány, 2015, Adopting the ritual stance: The role of opacity and context in ritual and everyday actions, Cognition, Brain, Behavior. an Interdisciplinary Journal, 145, 13
Kapitány, 2016, The ritual stance and the precaution system: The role of goal-demotion and opacity in ritual and everyday actions, Religion, Brain & Behavior, 1–16
Kenward, 2012, Over-imitating preschoolers believe unnecessary actions are normative and enforce their performance by a third party, Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 112, 195, 10.1016/j.jecp.2012.02.006
Kenward, 2011, Over-imitation is better explained by norm learning than by distorted causal learning, Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 278, 1239, 10.1098/rspb.2010.1399
Keupp, 2016, Rational over-imitation: Preschoolers consider material costs and copy causally irrelevant actions selectively, Cognition, Brain, Behavior. an Interdisciplinary Journal, 147, 85
Keupp, 2013, Why do children overimitate? Normativity is crucial, Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 116, 392, 10.1016/j.jecp.2013.07.002
Keupp, 2015, Over-imitation is not automatic: Context sensitivity in children’s overimitation and action interpretation of causally irrelevant actions, Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 130, 163, 10.1016/j.jecp.2014.10.005
Király, 2013, Beyond rational imitation: Learning arbitrary means actions from communicative demonstrations, Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 116, 471, 10.1016/j.jecp.2012.12.003
Kralik, 2012, When Less Is More: Evolutionary Origins of the Affect Heuristic, PLoS ONE, 7, 10.1371/journal.pone.0046240
Kruglanski, 2011, Intuitive and deliberate judgments are based on com- mon principles, Psychological Review, 118, 97, 10.1037/a0020762
Legare, 2017, Cumulative cultural learning: Development and diversity, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 114, 7877, 10.1073/pnas.1620743114
Legare, 2013, Cognitive consequences and constraints on reasoning about ritual, Religion, Brain & Behavior, 3, 63, 10.1080/2153599X.2012.736710
Legare, 2015, Imitation and innovation: The dual engines of cultural learning, Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 19, 688, 10.1016/j.tics.2015.08.005
Legare, 2012, Evaluating ritual efficacy: Evidence from the supernatural, Cognition, Brain, Behavior. an Interdisciplinary Journal, 124, 1
Legare, 2015, Imitative flexibility and the development of cultural learning, Cognition, Brain, Behavior. An Interdisciplinary Journal, 142, 351
Lewkowicz, 2014, Early experience and multisensory perceptual narrowing, Developmental Psychobiology, 56, 292, 10.1002/dev.21197
Lyons, 2011, The scope and limits of overimitation in the transmission of artefact culture, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 366, 1158, 10.1098/rstb.2010.0335
Lyons, 2007, The hidden structure of overimitation, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 104, 19751, 10.1073/pnas.0704452104
Marsh, 2013, Children with autism do not overimitate, Current Biology, 23, R266, 10.1016/j.cub.2013.02.036
Marsh, 2014, The social modulation of imitation fidelity in school-age children, PLoS ONE, 9, 1, 10.1371/journal.pone.0086127
McGuigan, 2015, The influence of peers on the tendency of 3- and 4-year-old children to over-imitate, Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 136, 42, 10.1016/j.jecp.2015.03.004
McGuigan, 2009, Emulation and ‘‘overemulation” in the social learning of causally opaque versus causally transparent tool use by 23- and 30-month-olds, Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 104, 367, 10.1016/j.jecp.2009.07.001
McGuigan, 2011, From over-imitation to super-copying: Adults imitate causally irrelevant aspects of tool use with higher fidelity than young children, British Journal of Psychology, 102, 1, 10.1348/000712610X493115
Morsanyi, K. J. H. S., & Evans, J. S. B. (2009). Heuristics and biases in autism: Less biased but not more logical, 1–6.
Nielsen, 2018, The social glue of cumulative culture and ritual behavior, Child Development Perspectives, 364, 2405
Nielsen, 2011, Imitation in young children: When who gets copied is more important than what gets copied, Developmental Psychology, 47, 1050, 10.1037/a0023866
Nielsen, 2012, When the transmission of culture is child’s play, PLoS ONE, 7, 1, 10.1371/journal.pone.0034066
Nielsen, 2010, Over-imitation in children with autism and Down syndrome, Australien Journal of Psychology, 62, 67, 10.1080/00049530902758613
Nielsen, 2011, Failure to find over-imitation in captive orangutans (Pongo pygmaeus): Implications for our understanding of cross-generation information transfer, 153
Nielsen, 2013, Object-directed imitation in children with high-functioning autism: Testing the social motivation hypothesis, Autism Research, 6, 23, 10.1002/aur.1261
Osman, 2013, A case study: Dual-process theories of higher cognition – commentary on Evans & Stanovich, Perspectives on Psychological Science, 8, 248, 10.1177/1745691613483475
Over, 2012, Putting the social into social learning: Explaining both selectivity and fidelity in children's copying behavior, Journal of Comparative Psychology, 126, 182, 10.1037/a0024555
Over, 2013, The social side of imitation, Child Development Perspectives, 7, 6, 10.1111/cdep.12006
Paulus, 2013, Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 116, 487, 10.1016/j.jecp.2012.10.009
Reyna, 2011, Dual processes in decision making and developmental neuroscience: A fuzzy-trace model, Developmental Review, 31, 180
Rogers, 2003, Imitation performance in toddlers with autism and those with other developmental disorders, Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 44, 763, 10.1111/1469-7610.00162
Rossano, 2012, The essential role of ritual in the transmission and reinforcement of social norms, Psychological Bulletin, 138, 529, 10.1037/a0027038
Sánchez-Amaro, 2019, Disentangling Great Apes' Decoy-Effect Bias in a Food Choice Task, Animal Behavior and Cognition, 6, 1, 10.26451/abc.06.03.05.2019
Santos, 2009, The evolution of rational and irrational economic behavior, 81
Schleihauf, 2018, Contrasting social and cognitive accounts on overimitation: The role of causal transparency and prior experiences, Child Development, 89, 1039, 10.1111/cdev.12780
Schleihauf, 2019, Minimal group formation influences on over-imitation, Cognitive Development, 50, 222, 10.1016/j.cogdev.2019.04.004
Slovic, P., Finucane, M., Peters, E., & MacGregor, D. G. (2002). The affect heuristic. In Heuristics and biases (pp. 1–20).
Spelke, 1990, Principles of object perception, Cognitive Science, 14, 29, 10.1207/s15516709cog1401_3
Stanovich, 2009, Distinguishing the reflective, algorithmic, and autonomous minds: Is it time for a tri-process theory?, 55
Striano, 2010, Seven- to 9-month-old infants use facial expressions to interpret others' actions, British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 24, 753, 10.1348/026151005X70319
Subiaul, 2014, Working memory constraints on imitation and emulation, Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 1–12
Taniguchi, 2017, The boundaries of overimitation in preschool children: Effects of target and tool use on imitation of irrelevant actions, Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 159, 83, 10.1016/j.jecp.2017.01.014
Tennie, 2012, Untrained Chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes schweinfurthii) Fail to Imitate Novel Actions, PLoS ONE, 7, e41548, 10.1371/journal.pone.0041548
Tinbergen, 1963, On aims and methods in ethology, Zeitschrift für Tierpsychologie, 20, 410, 10.1111/j.1439-0310.1963.tb01161.x
Tversky, 1981, The Framing of Decisions and the Psychology of Choice, Science, 211, 453, 10.1126/science.7455683
Varga, 2014, Beyond type 1 vs. type 2 processing: The tri-dimensional way, Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 1, 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00993
Vivanti, 2017, The social nature of overimitation: Insights from Autism and Williams syndrome, Cognition, 161, 10, 10.1016/j.cognition.2017.01.008
de Waal, 2008, Putting the Altruism Back into Altruism: The Evolution of Empathy, Annual Review of Psychology, 59, 279, 10.1146/annurev.psych.59.103006.093625
Watson-Jones, 2014, Task-specific effects of ostracism on imitative fidelity in early childhood, Evolution and Human Behavior, 1–7
Whiten, A., Horner, V., & Marshall-Pescini, S. (2005). Selective imitation in child and chimpanzee: A window on the construal of others' actions. In S. L. Hurley & N. Chater (Eds.), Perspectives on Imitation From Mirror Neurons to Memes (pp. 263–283).
Whiten, 2009, Emulation, imitation, over-imitation and the scope of culture for child and chimpanzee, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 364, 2417, 10.1098/rstb.2009.0069
Young, 2011, Imitation from 12 to 24 months in autism and typical development: A longitudinal Rasch analysis, Developmental Psychology, 47, 1565, 10.1037/a0025418
Yu, 2015, Understanding young children’s imitative behavior from an individual differences perspective, 2769
Zmyj, 2009, The development of rational imitation in 9- and 12-month-old infants, Infancy, 14, 131, 10.1080/15250000802569884
Zmyj, 2012, The peer model advantage in infants' imitation of familiar gestures performed by differently aged models, Front Psychol, 3, 252, 10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00252
Zmyj, 2010, The reliability of a model influences 14-month-olds' imitation, Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 106, 208, 10.1016/j.jecp.2010.03.002