A dual-process perspective on over-imitation

Developmental Review - Tập 55 - Trang 100896 - 2020
Hanna Schleihauf1,2,3, Stefanie Hoehl4
1Socials Origins Lab, Department of Psychology, University of California Berkeley, Berkeley, CA 94704, United States
2Department for Cognitive Ethology, Johann-Friedrich-Blumenbach-Institute for Zoology & Anthropology, University of Göttingen, 37073 Göttingen, Germany
3Cognitive Ethology Laboratory, German Primate Center – Leibniz Institute for Primate Research, 37077 Göttingen, Germany
4Department for Developmental Psychology, Faculty of Psychology, University of Vienna, 1010 Vienna, Austria

Tài liệu tham khảo

Bandura, A. (1986). Social foundations of thought and action. Prentice Hall. doi: 10.4135/9781446221129.n6. Bandura, 1999, 154 Bandura, 2001, Social cognitive theory: An agentic perspective, Annual Review of Psychology, 52, 1, 10.1146/annurev.psych.52.1.1 Bandura, 1971 Bekkering, 2000, Imitation of gestures in children is goal-directed, The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 53A, 153, 10.1080/713755872 Beran, 2017, To Err Is (Not Only) Human: Fallibility as a window into primate cognition, Comparative Cognition & Behavior Reviews, 12, 57, 10.3819/CCBR.2017.120006 Boyer, 2006, Precaution systems and ritualized behavior, Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 29, 635, 10.1017/S0140525X06009575 Brainerd, 2002, Fuzzy-trace theory and false memory, Current Directions in Psychological Science, 11, 164, 10.1111/1467-8721.00192 Brainerd, 2002, Are young children susceptible to the False-Memory Illusion?, Child Development, 73, 1363, 10.1111/1467-8624.00477 Brosnan, 2016, Reasoning on the Autism Spectrum: A dual process theory account, Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 46, 2115, 10.1007/s10803-016-2742-4 Bulbulia, 2011, Signalling theory and the evolution of religious cooperation, Religion, 41, 363, 10.1080/0048721X.2011.604508 Buttelmann, 2013, Selective imitation of in-group over out-group members in 14-month-old infants, Child Development, 84, 422, 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2012.01860.x Brugger, 2007, Doing the right thing: Infants’ selection of actions to imitate from observed event sequences, Child Development, 78, 806, 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2007.01034.x Call, 2006, Descartes' two errors: Reason and reflection in the great apes, Rational Animals?, 219, 10.1093/acprof:oso/9780198528272.003.0010 Carpenter, 1998, Fourteen- through 18-month-old infants differentially imitate intentional and accidental actions, Infant Behavior and Development, 21, 315, 10.1016/S0163-6383(98)90009-1 Chartrand, 1999, The Chameleon Effect: The perception-behavior link and social interaction, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 76, 893, 10.1037/0022-3514.76.6.893 Chartrand, 2009, Human mimicry, Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 41, 219, 10.1016/S0065-2601(08)00405-X Clay, 2017, Is overimitation a uniquely human phenomenon? Insights from human children as compared to bonobos, Child Development Clay, 2018, What drives young children to over-imitate? Investigating the effects of age, context, action type, and transitivity, Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 166, 520, 10.1016/j.jecp.2017.09.008 Dakin, 2005, Vagaries of visual perception in autism, Neuron, 48, 497, 10.1016/j.neuron.2005.10.018 De Neys, 2006, Dual processing in reasoning, Psychological Science, 17, 428, 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2006.01723.x DiYanni, 2008, Using a bad tool with good intention: Young children’s imitation of adults’ questionable choices, Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 101, 241, 10.1016/j.jecp.2008.05.002 Eilam, 2006, Rituals, stereotypy and compulsive behavior in animals and humans, Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 30, 456, 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2005.08.003 Etten, 2015, Does impaired social motivation drive imitation deficits in children with autism spectrum disorder?, Review Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 2, 310, 10.1007/s40489-015-0054-9 Evans, 2017, Selective copying of the majority suggests children are broadly “optimal-” rather than ‘over-’ imitators, Developmental Science, 70, e12637 Evans, 2010, Intuition and reasoning: A dual-process perspective, Psychological Inquiry, 21, 313, 10.1080/1047840X.2010.521057 Evans, 2005, Rapid responding increases belief bias: Evidence for the dual-process theory of reasoning, Thinking & Reasoning, 11, 382, 10.1080/13546780542000005 Evans, 2013, Dual-Process theories of higher cognition, Perspectives on Psychological Science, 8, 223, 10.1177/1745691612460685 Flynn, 2012, Investigating the mechanisms of cultural acquisition - How pervasive is overimitation in adults?, Social Psychology, 43, 185, 10.1027/1864-9335/a000119 Frankish, 2010, Dual-process and dual-system theories of reasoning, Philosophy Compass, 5, 914, 10.1111/j.1747-9991.2010.00330.x Gergely, 2003, Teleological reasoning in infancy: the naïve theory of rational action, Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7, 287, 10.1016/S1364-6613(03)00128-1 Gergely, 2002, Rational imitation in preverbal infants, Nature, 415, 755, 10.1038/415755a Gonsiorowski, 2016, Brief report: Imitation of object-directed acts in young children with autism spectrum disorders, Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 46, 691, 10.1007/s10803-015-2596-1 Gopnik, 2015, When younger learners can be better (or at least more open-minded) than older ones, Current Directions in Psychological Science, 24, 87, 10.1177/0963721414556653 Hamilton, A. F. (n.d.). The neurocognitive mechanisms of imitation. Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences, 3, 63–67. doi: 10.1016/j.cobeha.2015.01.011. Happé, 2006, The Weak Coherence Account: Detail-focused cognitive style in Autism Spectrum Disorders, Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 36, 5, 10.1007/s10803-005-0039-0 Heyes, 2011, Automatic imitation, Psychological Bulletin, 137, 463, 10.1037/a0022288 Hilbrink, 2013, Selective and faithful imitation at 12 and 15 months, Developmental Science, 16, 828 Hoehl, 2019, Over-imitation - A review and appraisal of a decade of research, Developmental Review, 51, 90, 10.1016/j.dr.2018.12.002 Hoehl, 2014, The role of social interaction and pedagogical cues for eliciting and reducing overimitation in preschoolers, Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 122, 122, 10.1016/j.jecp.2013.12.012 Horner, 2005, Causal knowledge and imitation/emulation switching in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) and children (Homo sapiens), Animal Cognition, 8, 164, 10.1007/s10071-004-0239-6 Howard, 2015, Infants’ and young children's imitation of linguistic in-group and out-group informants, Child Development, 86, 259, 10.1111/cdev.12299 Johnson, 2010, Interactive Specialization: A domain-general framework for human functional brain development?, Accident Analysis and Prevention, 1, 7 Kahneman, 2011, Thinking, fast and slow, Farrar, Straus and Giroux. Kahneman, 1990, Experimental tests of the endowment effect and the Coase theorem, Journal of Political Economy, 98, 1325, 10.1086/261737 Kapitány, 2015, Adopting the ritual stance: The role of opacity and context in ritual and everyday actions, Cognition, Brain, Behavior. an Interdisciplinary Journal, 145, 13 Kapitány, 2016, The ritual stance and the precaution system: The role of goal-demotion and opacity in ritual and everyday actions, Religion, Brain & Behavior, 1–16 Kenward, 2012, Over-imitating preschoolers believe unnecessary actions are normative and enforce their performance by a third party, Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 112, 195, 10.1016/j.jecp.2012.02.006 Kenward, 2011, Over-imitation is better explained by norm learning than by distorted causal learning, Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 278, 1239, 10.1098/rspb.2010.1399 Keupp, 2016, Rational over-imitation: Preschoolers consider material costs and copy causally irrelevant actions selectively, Cognition, Brain, Behavior. an Interdisciplinary Journal, 147, 85 Keupp, 2013, Why do children overimitate? Normativity is crucial, Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 116, 392, 10.1016/j.jecp.2013.07.002 Keupp, 2015, Over-imitation is not automatic: Context sensitivity in children’s overimitation and action interpretation of causally irrelevant actions, Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 130, 163, 10.1016/j.jecp.2014.10.005 Király, 2013, Beyond rational imitation: Learning arbitrary means actions from communicative demonstrations, Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 116, 471, 10.1016/j.jecp.2012.12.003 Kralik, 2012, When Less Is More: Evolutionary Origins of the Affect Heuristic, PLoS ONE, 7, 10.1371/journal.pone.0046240 Kruglanski, 2011, Intuitive and deliberate judgments are based on com- mon principles, Psychological Review, 118, 97, 10.1037/a0020762 Legare, 2017, Cumulative cultural learning: Development and diversity, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 114, 7877, 10.1073/pnas.1620743114 Legare, 2013, Cognitive consequences and constraints on reasoning about ritual, Religion, Brain & Behavior, 3, 63, 10.1080/2153599X.2012.736710 Legare, 2015, Imitation and innovation: The dual engines of cultural learning, Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 19, 688, 10.1016/j.tics.2015.08.005 Legare, 2012, Evaluating ritual efficacy: Evidence from the supernatural, Cognition, Brain, Behavior. an Interdisciplinary Journal, 124, 1 Legare, 2015, Imitative flexibility and the development of cultural learning, Cognition, Brain, Behavior. An Interdisciplinary Journal, 142, 351 Lewkowicz, 2014, Early experience and multisensory perceptual narrowing, Developmental Psychobiology, 56, 292, 10.1002/dev.21197 Lyons, 2011, The scope and limits of overimitation in the transmission of artefact culture, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 366, 1158, 10.1098/rstb.2010.0335 Lyons, 2007, The hidden structure of overimitation, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 104, 19751, 10.1073/pnas.0704452104 Marsh, 2013, Children with autism do not overimitate, Current Biology, 23, R266, 10.1016/j.cub.2013.02.036 Marsh, 2014, The social modulation of imitation fidelity in school-age children, PLoS ONE, 9, 1, 10.1371/journal.pone.0086127 McGuigan, 2015, The influence of peers on the tendency of 3- and 4-year-old children to over-imitate, Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 136, 42, 10.1016/j.jecp.2015.03.004 McGuigan, 2009, Emulation and ‘‘overemulation” in the social learning of causally opaque versus causally transparent tool use by 23- and 30-month-olds, Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 104, 367, 10.1016/j.jecp.2009.07.001 McGuigan, 2011, From over-imitation to super-copying: Adults imitate causally irrelevant aspects of tool use with higher fidelity than young children, British Journal of Psychology, 102, 1, 10.1348/000712610X493115 Morsanyi, K. J. H. S., & Evans, J. S. B. (2009). Heuristics and biases in autism: Less biased but not more logical, 1–6. Nielsen, 2018, The social glue of cumulative culture and ritual behavior, Child Development Perspectives, 364, 2405 Nielsen, 2011, Imitation in young children: When who gets copied is more important than what gets copied, Developmental Psychology, 47, 1050, 10.1037/a0023866 Nielsen, 2012, When the transmission of culture is child’s play, PLoS ONE, 7, 1, 10.1371/journal.pone.0034066 Nielsen, 2010, Over-imitation in children with autism and Down syndrome, Australien Journal of Psychology, 62, 67, 10.1080/00049530902758613 Nielsen, 2011, Failure to find over-imitation in captive orangutans (Pongo pygmaeus): Implications for our understanding of cross-generation information transfer, 153 Nielsen, 2013, Object-directed imitation in children with high-functioning autism: Testing the social motivation hypothesis, Autism Research, 6, 23, 10.1002/aur.1261 Osman, 2013, A case study: Dual-process theories of higher cognition – commentary on Evans & Stanovich, Perspectives on Psychological Science, 8, 248, 10.1177/1745691613483475 Over, 2012, Putting the social into social learning: Explaining both selectivity and fidelity in children's copying behavior, Journal of Comparative Psychology, 126, 182, 10.1037/a0024555 Over, 2013, The social side of imitation, Child Development Perspectives, 7, 6, 10.1111/cdep.12006 Paulus, 2013, Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 116, 487, 10.1016/j.jecp.2012.10.009 Reyna, 2011, Dual processes in decision making and developmental neuroscience: A fuzzy-trace model, Developmental Review, 31, 180 Rogers, 2003, Imitation performance in toddlers with autism and those with other developmental disorders, Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 44, 763, 10.1111/1469-7610.00162 Rossano, 2012, The essential role of ritual in the transmission and reinforcement of social norms, Psychological Bulletin, 138, 529, 10.1037/a0027038 Sánchez-Amaro, 2019, Disentangling Great Apes' Decoy-Effect Bias in a Food Choice Task, Animal Behavior and Cognition, 6, 1, 10.26451/abc.06.03.05.2019 Santos, 2009, The evolution of rational and irrational economic behavior, 81 Schleihauf, 2018, Contrasting social and cognitive accounts on overimitation: The role of causal transparency and prior experiences, Child Development, 89, 1039, 10.1111/cdev.12780 Schleihauf, 2019, Minimal group formation influences on over-imitation, Cognitive Development, 50, 222, 10.1016/j.cogdev.2019.04.004 Slovic, P., Finucane, M., Peters, E., & MacGregor, D. G. (2002). The affect heuristic. In Heuristics and biases (pp. 1–20). Spelke, 1990, Principles of object perception, Cognitive Science, 14, 29, 10.1207/s15516709cog1401_3 Stanovich, 2009, Distinguishing the reflective, algorithmic, and autonomous minds: Is it time for a tri-process theory?, 55 Striano, 2010, Seven- to 9-month-old infants use facial expressions to interpret others' actions, British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 24, 753, 10.1348/026151005X70319 Subiaul, 2014, Working memory constraints on imitation and emulation, Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 1–12 Taniguchi, 2017, The boundaries of overimitation in preschool children: Effects of target and tool use on imitation of irrelevant actions, Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 159, 83, 10.1016/j.jecp.2017.01.014 Tennie, 2012, Untrained Chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes schweinfurthii) Fail to Imitate Novel Actions, PLoS ONE, 7, e41548, 10.1371/journal.pone.0041548 Tinbergen, 1963, On aims and methods in ethology, Zeitschrift für Tierpsychologie, 20, 410, 10.1111/j.1439-0310.1963.tb01161.x Tversky, 1981, The Framing of Decisions and the Psychology of Choice, Science, 211, 453, 10.1126/science.7455683 Varga, 2014, Beyond type 1 vs. type 2 processing: The tri-dimensional way, Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 1, 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00993 Vivanti, 2017, The social nature of overimitation: Insights from Autism and Williams syndrome, Cognition, 161, 10, 10.1016/j.cognition.2017.01.008 de Waal, 2008, Putting the Altruism Back into Altruism: The Evolution of Empathy, Annual Review of Psychology, 59, 279, 10.1146/annurev.psych.59.103006.093625 Watson-Jones, 2014, Task-specific effects of ostracism on imitative fidelity in early childhood, Evolution and Human Behavior, 1–7 Whiten, A., Horner, V., & Marshall-Pescini, S. (2005). Selective imitation in child and chimpanzee: A window on the construal of others' actions. In S. L. Hurley & N. Chater (Eds.), Perspectives on Imitation From Mirror Neurons to Memes (pp. 263–283). Whiten, 2009, Emulation, imitation, over-imitation and the scope of culture for child and chimpanzee, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 364, 2417, 10.1098/rstb.2009.0069 Young, 2011, Imitation from 12 to 24 months in autism and typical development: A longitudinal Rasch analysis, Developmental Psychology, 47, 1565, 10.1037/a0025418 Yu, 2015, Understanding young children’s imitative behavior from an individual differences perspective, 2769 Zmyj, 2009, The development of rational imitation in 9- and 12-month-old infants, Infancy, 14, 131, 10.1080/15250000802569884 Zmyj, 2012, The peer model advantage in infants' imitation of familiar gestures performed by differently aged models, Front Psychol, 3, 252, 10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00252 Zmyj, 2010, The reliability of a model influences 14-month-olds' imitation, Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 106, 208, 10.1016/j.jecp.2010.03.002