A configural theory of attention and associative learning
Tóm tắt
Từ khóa
Tài liệu tham khảo
Atkinson, R. C., & Estes, W. K. (1963). Stimulus sampling theory. In R. D. Luce, R. B. Bush, & E. Galanter (Eds.), Handbook of mathematical psychology (Vol. 3, pp. 121–268). New York: Wiley.
Bellingham, W. P., Gillette-Bellingham, K., & Kehoe, E. J. (1985). Summation and configuration in patterning schedules with the rabbit. Animal Learning and Behavior, 13, 152–164.
Bouton, M. E., & Peck, C. A. (1989). Context effects on conditioning, extinction, and reinstatement in an appetitive conditioning preparation. Animal Learning and Behavior, 17, 188–198.
Brandon, S. E., Vogel, E. H., & Wagner, A. R. (2000). A componential view of configural cues in generalization and discrimination in Pavlovian conditioning. Behavioural Brain Research, 110, 67–72.
Dopson, J. C., Esber, G. R., & Pearce, J. M. (2010). Differences in the associability of relevant and irrelevant stimuli. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Animal Behavior Processes, 36, 258–267. doi: 10.1037/a0016588
Dopson, J. C., Esber, G. R., & Pearce, J. M. (2011). Changes in attention to an irrelevant cue that accompanies a negative patterning discrimination. Learning & Behavior, 39, 336–349. doi: 10.3758/s13420-011-0029-3
Dopson, J. C., Williams, N. A., Esber, G. R., & Pearce, J. M. (2010). Stimuli that signal the absence of reinforcement are paid more attention than are irrelevant stimuli. Learning & Behavior, 38, 337–347. doi: 10.3758/LB.38.4.337
George, D. N., & Pearce, J. M. (1999). Acquired distinctiveness is controlled by stimulus relevance not correlation with reward. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Animal Behavior Processes, 25, 363–373.
Hall, G., & Channell, S. (1985). Differential effects of contextual change on latent inhibition and on the habituation of an orienting response. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Animal Behavior Processes, 11, 470–481. doi: 10.1037/0097-7403.11.3.470
Hall, G., & Pearce, J. M. (1979). Latent inhibition of a CS during CS–US pairings. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Animal Behavior Processes, 5, 31–42. doi: 10.1037/0097-7403.5.1.31
Haselgrove, M., Esber, G. R., Pearce, J. M., & Jones, P. M. (2010). Two kinds of attention in Pavlovian conditioning: Evidence for a hybrid model of learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Animal Behavior Processes, 36, 456–470.
Kaye, H., & Pearce, J. M. (1984). The strength of the orienting response during Pavlovian conditioning. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Animal Behavior Processes, 10, 90–109.
Kinder, A., & Lachnit, H. (2003). Similarity and discrimination in human Pavlovian conditioning. Psychophysiology, 40, 226–234. doi: 10.1111/1469-8986.00024
Le Pelley, M. E. (2004). The role of associative history in models of associative learning: A selective review and a hybrid model. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 57B, 193–243. doi: 10.1080/02724990344000141
Lubow, R. E., & Moore, A. U. (1959). Latent inhibition: The effect of nonreinforced pre-exposure to the conditional stimulus. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 52, 415–419. doi: 10.1037/h0046700
Mackintosh, N. J. (1975). A theory of attention: Variations in the associability of stimuli with reinforcement. Psychological Review, 82, 276–298. doi: 10.1037/h0076778
Mackintosh, N. J. (1976). Overshadowing and stimulus intensity. Animal Learning and Behavior, 4, 186–192.
Mackintosh, N. J., & Little, L. (1969). Intradimensional and extradimensional shift learning by pigeons. Psychonomic Science, 14, 5–6.
Pavlov, I. P. (1927). Conditioned reflexes (G. V. Anrep, Trans.). London, U.K.: Oxford University Press.
Pearce, J. M. (1987). A model for stimulus generalization for Pavlovian conditioning. Psychological Review, 94, 61–73. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.94.1.61
Pearce, J. M. (1994). Similarity and discrimination: A selective review and a connectionist model. Psychological Review, 101, 587–607. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.101.4.587
Pearce, J. M. (2002). Evaluation and development of a connectionist theory of configural learning. Animal Learning and Behavior, 30, 73–95.
Pearce, J. M., Esber, G. R., George, D. N., & Haselgrove, M. (2008). The nature of discrimination learning in pigeons. Learning & Behavior, 36, 188–199.
Pearce, J. M., George, D. N., & Redhead, E. S. (1998). The role of attention in the solution of conditional discriminations. In N. A. Schmajuk & P. C. Holland (Eds.), Occasion setting: Associative learning and cognition in animals (pp. 249–275). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.
Pearce, J. M., & Hall, G. (1980). A model for Pavlovian learning: Variations in the effectiveness of conditioned but not of unconditioned stimuli. Psychological Review, 87, 532–552. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.87.6.532
Pearce, J. M., Kaye, H., & Hall, G. (1982). Predictive accuracy and stimulus associability: Development of a model for Pavlovian learning. In M. L. Commons, R. J. Herrnstein, & A. R. Wagner (Eds.), Quantitative analyses of behavior (Vol. 3, pp. 241–256). Cambridge: Ballinger.
Pearce, J. M., & Mackintosh, N. J. (2010). Two theories of attention: A review and a possible integration. In C. J. Mitchell & M. E. Le Pelley (Eds.), Attention and associative learning: From brain to behaviour (pp. 11–39). Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Pearce, J. M., & Redhead, E. S. (1993). The influence of an irrelevant stimulus on two discriminations. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Animal Behavior Processes, 19, 180–190.
Reberg, D., & LeClerc, R. (1977). A feature positive effect in conditioned suppression. Animal Learning & Behavior, 5, 143–147.
Rescorla, R. A. (1969). Pavlovian conditioned inhibition. Psychological Bulletin, 72, 77–94. doi: 10.1037/h0027760
Rescorla, R. A. (2000). Associative change in excitors and inhibitors differ when they are conditioned in compound. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Animal Behavior Processes, 26, 428–438.
Sutherland, N. S., & Mackintosh, N. J. (1971). Mechanisms of animal discrimination learning. New York: Academic Press.
Swan, J. A., & Pearce, J. M. (1988). The orienting response as an index of stimulus associability in rats. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Animal Behavior Processes, 14, 292–301.
Swartzentruber, D., & Bouton, M. E. (1986). Contextual control of negative transfer produced by prior CS-US pairings. Learning and Motivation, 17, 366–385.
Thomas, B. L., Larsen, N., & Ayres, J. J. B. (2003). Role of context similarity in ABA, ABC, and AAB renewal paradigms: Implications for theories of renewal and for treating human phobias. Learning and Motivation, 34, 410–436.
Williams, D. A., Mehta, R., & Dumont, J. L. (2004). Conditions favoring superconditioning of irrelevant conditioned stimuli. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Animal Behavior Processes, 30, 148–159.
Williams, D. A., Mehta, R., Poworonzyk, T. M., Oriehel, J. S., George, D. N., & Pearce, J. M. (2002). Acquisition of superexcitatory properties by an irrelevant background stimulus. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Animal Behavior Processes, 28, 284–297.
Wilson, P. N., Boumphrey, P., & Pearce, J. M. (1992). Restoration of the orienting response to a light by a change in its predictive accuracy. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 44B, 17–36. doi: 10.1080/02724999208250600