A configural theory of attention and associative learning

David N. George1, John M. Pearce2
1Department of Psychology, University of Hull, Hull HU6 7RX, UK
2School of Psychology, Cardiff University, Cardiff CF10 3AT, UK

Tóm tắt

Từ khóa


Tài liệu tham khảo

Atkinson, R. C., & Estes, W. K. (1963). Stimulus sampling theory. In R. D. Luce, R. B. Bush, & E. Galanter (Eds.), Handbook of mathematical psychology (Vol. 3, pp. 121–268). New York: Wiley.

Bellingham, W. P., Gillette-Bellingham, K., & Kehoe, E. J. (1985). Summation and configuration in patterning schedules with the rabbit. Animal Learning and Behavior, 13, 152–164.

Bouton, M. E., & Peck, C. A. (1989). Context effects on conditioning, extinction, and reinstatement in an appetitive conditioning preparation. Animal Learning and Behavior, 17, 188–198.

Brandon, S. E., Vogel, E. H., & Wagner, A. R. (2000). A componential view of configural cues in generalization and discrimination in Pavlovian conditioning. Behavioural Brain Research, 110, 67–72.

Dopson, J. C., Esber, G. R., & Pearce, J. M. (2010). Differences in the associability of relevant and irrelevant stimuli. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Animal Behavior Processes, 36, 258–267. doi: 10.1037/a0016588

Dopson, J. C., Esber, G. R., & Pearce, J. M. (2011). Changes in attention to an irrelevant cue that accompanies a negative patterning discrimination. Learning & Behavior, 39, 336–349. doi: 10.3758/s13420-011-0029-3

Dopson, J. C., Williams, N. A., Esber, G. R., & Pearce, J. M. (2010). Stimuli that signal the absence of reinforcement are paid more attention than are irrelevant stimuli. Learning & Behavior, 38, 337–347. doi: 10.3758/LB.38.4.337

George, D. N., & Pearce, J. M. (1999). Acquired distinctiveness is controlled by stimulus relevance not correlation with reward. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Animal Behavior Processes, 25, 363–373.

Hall, G., & Channell, S. (1985). Differential effects of contextual change on latent inhibition and on the habituation of an orienting response. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Animal Behavior Processes, 11, 470–481. doi: 10.1037/0097-7403.11.3.470

Hall, G., & Pearce, J. M. (1979). Latent inhibition of a CS during CS–US pairings. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Animal Behavior Processes, 5, 31–42. doi: 10.1037/0097-7403.5.1.31

Haselgrove, M., Esber, G. R., Pearce, J. M., & Jones, P. M. (2010). Two kinds of attention in Pavlovian conditioning: Evidence for a hybrid model of learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Animal Behavior Processes, 36, 456–470.

Kaye, H., & Pearce, J. M. (1984). The strength of the orienting response during Pavlovian conditioning. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Animal Behavior Processes, 10, 90–109.

Kinder, A., & Lachnit, H. (2003). Similarity and discrimination in human Pavlovian conditioning. Psychophysiology, 40, 226–234. doi: 10.1111/1469-8986.00024

Le Pelley, M. E. (2004). The role of associative history in models of associative learning: A selective review and a hybrid model. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 57B, 193–243. doi: 10.1080/02724990344000141

Lubow, R. E., & Moore, A. U. (1959). Latent inhibition: The effect of nonreinforced pre-exposure to the conditional stimulus. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 52, 415–419. doi: 10.1037/h0046700

Mackintosh, N. J. (1975). A theory of attention: Variations in the associability of stimuli with reinforcement. Psychological Review, 82, 276–298. doi: 10.1037/h0076778

Mackintosh, N. J. (1976). Overshadowing and stimulus intensity. Animal Learning and Behavior, 4, 186–192.

Mackintosh, N. J., & Little, L. (1969). Intradimensional and extradimensional shift learning by pigeons. Psychonomic Science, 14, 5–6.

Pavlov, I. P. (1927). Conditioned reflexes (G. V. Anrep, Trans.). London, U.K.: Oxford University Press.

Pearce, J. M. (1987). A model for stimulus generalization for Pavlovian conditioning. Psychological Review, 94, 61–73. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.94.1.61

Pearce, J. M. (1994). Similarity and discrimination: A selective review and a connectionist model. Psychological Review, 101, 587–607. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.101.4.587

Pearce, J. M. (2002). Evaluation and development of a connectionist theory of configural learning. Animal Learning and Behavior, 30, 73–95.

Pearce, J. M., Esber, G. R., George, D. N., & Haselgrove, M. (2008). The nature of discrimination learning in pigeons. Learning & Behavior, 36, 188–199.

Pearce, J. M., George, D. N., & Redhead, E. S. (1998). The role of attention in the solution of conditional discriminations. In N. A. Schmajuk & P. C. Holland (Eds.), Occasion setting: Associative learning and cognition in animals (pp. 249–275). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.

Pearce, J. M., & Hall, G. (1980). A model for Pavlovian learning: Variations in the effectiveness of conditioned but not of unconditioned stimuli. Psychological Review, 87, 532–552. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.87.6.532

Pearce, J. M., Kaye, H., & Hall, G. (1982). Predictive accuracy and stimulus associability: Development of a model for Pavlovian learning. In M. L. Commons, R. J. Herrnstein, & A. R. Wagner (Eds.), Quantitative analyses of behavior (Vol. 3, pp. 241–256). Cambridge: Ballinger.

Pearce, J. M., & Mackintosh, N. J. (2010). Two theories of attention: A review and a possible integration. In C. J. Mitchell & M. E. Le Pelley (Eds.), Attention and associative learning: From brain to behaviour (pp. 11–39). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Pearce, J. M., & Redhead, E. S. (1993). The influence of an irrelevant stimulus on two discriminations. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Animal Behavior Processes, 19, 180–190.

Reberg, D., & LeClerc, R. (1977). A feature positive effect in conditioned suppression. Animal Learning & Behavior, 5, 143–147.

Rescorla, R. A. (1969). Pavlovian conditioned inhibition. Psychological Bulletin, 72, 77–94. doi: 10.1037/h0027760

Rescorla, R. A. (2000). Associative change in excitors and inhibitors differ when they are conditioned in compound. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Animal Behavior Processes, 26, 428–438.

Sutherland, N. S., & Mackintosh, N. J. (1971). Mechanisms of animal discrimination learning. New York: Academic Press.

Swan, J. A., & Pearce, J. M. (1988). The orienting response as an index of stimulus associability in rats. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Animal Behavior Processes, 14, 292–301.

Swartzentruber, D., & Bouton, M. E. (1986). Contextual control of negative transfer produced by prior CS-US pairings. Learning and Motivation, 17, 366–385.

Thomas, B. L., Larsen, N., & Ayres, J. J. B. (2003). Role of context similarity in ABA, ABC, and AAB renewal paradigms: Implications for theories of renewal and for treating human phobias. Learning and Motivation, 34, 410–436.

Williams, D. A., Mehta, R., & Dumont, J. L. (2004). Conditions favoring superconditioning of irrelevant conditioned stimuli. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Animal Behavior Processes, 30, 148–159.

Williams, D. A., Mehta, R., Poworonzyk, T. M., Oriehel, J. S., George, D. N., & Pearce, J. M. (2002). Acquisition of superexcitatory properties by an irrelevant background stimulus. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Animal Behavior Processes, 28, 284–297.

Wilson, P. N., Boumphrey, P., & Pearce, J. M. (1992). Restoration of the orienting response to a light by a change in its predictive accuracy. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 44B, 17–36. doi: 10.1080/02724999208250600