A Distributed Representation of Temporal Context

Journal of Mathematical Psychology - Tập 46 Số 3 - Trang 269-299 - 2002
Marc W. Howard1, Michael J. Kahana1
1Brandeis University

Tóm tắt

Từ khóa


Tài liệu tham khảo

Anderson, 1972, Recognition and retrieval processes in free recall, Psychological Review, 79, 97, 10.1037/h0033773

Asch, 1962, The principle of associative symmetry, Proceedings of the American Philosophical Society, 106, 135

Atkinson, 1968, Human memory: A proposed system and its control processes, 89, 10.1016/S0079-7421(08)60422-3

Baddeley, 1977, Recency reexamined, 647

Bahrick, 1970, Two-phase model for prompted recall, Psychological Review, 77, 215, 10.1037/h0029099

Bjork, 1974, Recency-sensitive retrieval processes in long-term free recall, Cognitive Psychology, 6, 173, 10.1016/0010-0285(74)90009-7

Brodie, 1977, Effects of presentation time on nominal and functional serial position curves in free recall, Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 16, 185, 10.1016/S0022-5371(77)80046-7

Brown, 2000, Oscillator-based memory for serial order, Psychological Review, 107, 127, 10.1037/0033-295X.107.1.127

Burgess, 1992, Toward a network model of articulatory loop, Journal of Memory and Language, 31, 429, 10.1016/0749-596X(92)90022-P

Burgess, 1999, Memory for serial order: A network model of the phonological loop and its timing, Psychological Review, 106, 551, 10.1037/0033-295X.106.3.551

Chance, 1997, Testing the role of associative interference and compound cues in sequence memory, 599

Chappell, 1994, An autoassociative neural network for sparse representations: Analysis and application to models of recognition and cued recall, Psychological Review, 101, 103, 10.1037/0033-295X.101.1.103

Cleeremans, 1993

Cleeremans, 1991, Learning the structure of event sequences, Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 120, 235, 10.1037/0096-3445.120.3.235

Ebbinghaus, 1913

Elman, 1990, Finding structure in time, Cognitive Science, 14, 179, 10.1207/s15516709cog1402_1

Estes, 1955, Statistical theory of spontaneous recovery and regression, Psychological Review, 62, 145, 10.1037/h0048509

Estes, 1972, An associative basis for coding and organization in memory, 161

Falkenberg, 1972, Recall improves in short-term memory the more recall context resembles learning context, Journal of Experimental Psychology, 93, 39, 10.1037/h0033287

Friendly, 1982, The Toronto Word Pool: Norms for imagery, concreteness, orthographic variables, and grammatical usage for 1,080 words, Behavior Research Methods and Instrumentation, 14, 375, 10.3758/BF03203275

Gillund, 1984, A retrieval model for both recognition and recall, Psychological Review, 91, 1, 10.1037/0033-295X.91.1.1

Glanzer, 1972, Storage mechanisms in recall, 129, 10.1016/S0079-7421(08)60441-7

Glanzer, 1969, The removal of items from short-term storage, Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 8, 435, 10.1016/S0022-5371(69)80087-3

Glenberg, 1980, A two-process account of long-term serial position effects, Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and Memory, 6, 355

Glenberg, 1986, A temporal distinctiveness theory of recency and modality effects, Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory & Cognition, 12, 3

Grossberg, 1978, Behavioral contrast in short-term memory: serial binary memory models or parallel continuous memory models?, Journal of Mathematical Psychology, 17, 199, 10.1016/0022-2496(78)90016-0

Henson, 1996, Unchained memory: Error patterns rule out chaining models of immediate serial recall, The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology A, 49, 80, 10.1080/713755612

Henson, 1998, Short-term memory for serial order: The start-end model, Cognitive Psychology, 36, 73, 10.1006/cogp.1998.0685

Howard, 1999, Contextual variability and serial position effects in free recall, Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 25, 923

Johnson, 1991, A distinctiveness model of serial learning, Psychological Review, 98, 204, 10.1037/0033-295X.98.2.204

Jordan, 1986, An introduction to linear algebra in parallel distributed processing, 365

Kahana, 1996, Associative retrieval processes in free recall, Memory & Cognition, 24, 103, 10.3758/BF03197276

Kahana, 1999, Associative symmetry and memory theory, Manuscript submitted for publication

Kahana, M. J, &, Caplan, J. B. (in press), Associative asymmetry in probed recall of serial lists, Memory & Cognition.

Kintsch, 1970, Models for free recall and recognition

Laming, 1999, Testing the idea of distinct storage mechanisms in memory, International Journal of Psychology, 34, 419, 10.1080/002075999399774

Lee, 1977, Order and position in primary memory for letter strings, Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 16, 395, 10.1016/S0022-5371(77)80036-4

Lee, 1981, Item and order information in short-term memory: Evidence for multilevel perturbation processes, Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and Memory, 7, 149

Lewandowsky, 1989, Memory for serial order, Psychological Review, 96, 25, 10.1037/0033-295X.96.1.25

Li, 1993, Intralist distractors and recall direction: Constraints on models of memory for serial order, Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 19, 895

Li, 1995, Forward and backward recall: Different retrieval processes, Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 21, 837

Loftus, 1994, Using confidence intervals in within-subject designs, Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 1, 476, 10.3758/BF03210951

Marshall, 1972, The effects of the elimination of rehearsal on primacy and recency, Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 11, 649, 10.1016/S0022-5371(72)80049-5

McGeoch, 1936, The direction and extent of intraserial associations at recall, American Journal of Psychology, 48, 221, 10.2307/1415745

Mensink, 1988, A model for interference and forgetting, Psychological Review, 95, 434, 10.1037/0033-295X.95.4.434

Mensink, 1989, A model for contextual fluctuation, Journal of Mathematical Psychology, 33, 172, 10.1016/0022-2496(89)90029-1

Monsell, 1978, Recency, immediate recognition memory, and reaction time, Cognitive Psychology, 10, 465, 10.1016/0010-0285(78)90008-7

Murdock, 1962, The serial position effect of free recall, Journal of Experimental Psychology, 64, 482, 10.1037/h0045106

Murdock, 1963, Short-term memory and paired-associate learning, Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 2, 320, 10.1016/S0022-5371(63)80100-0

Murdock, 1997, Context and mediators in a theory of distributed associative memory (TODAM2), Psychological Review, 1997, 839, 10.1037/0033-295X.104.4.839

Murdock, 1993, Analysis of the list strength effect, Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 19, 689

Murdock, 1978, Controlled rehearsal in single-trial free recall, Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 17, 309, 10.1016/S0022-5371(78)90201-3

Murdock, 1970, Interresponse times in single-trial free recall, Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 86, 263

Murdock, B. B., Smith, D., & Bai, J.Judgements of frequency and recency in a distributed memory model. Journal of Mathematical Psychology, 45, 564–602.

Nairne, 1997, Positional distinctiveness and the ratio rule in free recall, Journal of Memory and Language, 37, 155, 10.1006/jmla.1997.2513

Neath, 1993, Distinctiveness and serial position effects in recognition, Memory and Cognition, 21, 689, 10.3758/BF03197199

Neath, 1990, Schedules of presentation and temporal distinctiveness in human memory, Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 16, 316

Neath, 1996, Distinctiveness and very short-term serial position effects, Memory, 4, 225, 10.1080/09658211.1996.9753032

Nelder, 1965, A simplex method for function minimization, Computer Journal, 7, 308, 10.1093/comjnl/7.4.308

Nosofsky, 1986, Attention, similarity, and the identification-categorization relationship, Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 115, 39, 10.1037/0096-3445.115.1.39

Nosofsky, 1987, Attention and learning processes in the identification and categorization of integral stimuli, Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 13, 87

Page, 1998, The primacy model: A new model of immediate serial recall, Psychological Review, 105, 761, 10.1037/0033-295X.105.4.761-781

Raaijmakers, 1980, SAM: A theory of probabilistic search of associative memory, 207, 10.1016/S0079-7421(08)60162-0

Raaijmakers, 1981, Order effects in recall, 403

Raaijmakers, 1981, Search of associative memory, Psychological Review, 88, 93, 10.1037/0033-295X.88.2.93

Raskin, 1937, The strength and direction of associations formed in the learning of nonsense syllables, Journal of Experimental Psychology, 20, 381, 10.1037/h0061612

Rizzuto, 2001, An autoassociative model of paired-associate learning, Neural Computation, 13, 1835, 10.1162/089976601750399317

Rohrer, 1994, An analysis of latency and interresponse time in free recall, Memory & Cognition, 22, 511, 10.3758/BF03198390

Rundus, 1971, An analysis of rehearsal processes in free recall, Journal of Experimental Psychology, 89, 63, 10.1037/h0031185

Rundus, 1980, Maintenance rehearsal and long-term recency, Memory & Cognition, 8, 226, 10.3758/BF03197610

Shepard, 1987, Toward a universal law of generalization for psychological science, Science, 237, 1317, 10.1126/science.3629243

Shiffrin, 1970, Forgetting: Trace erosion or retrieval failure?, Science, 168, 1601, 10.1126/science.168.3939.1601

Shiffrin, 1970, Memory search, 374

Shiffrin, 1997, A model for recognition memory: REM—retrieving effectively from memory, Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 4, 145, 10.3758/BF03209391

Tan, 2000, A recency-based account of the primacy effect in free recall, Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 26, 1589

Tulving, 1968, Theoretical issues in free recall, 2

Watkins, 1989, Recency effect in recall of a word list when an immediate memory task is performed after each word presentation, American Journal of Psychology, 102, 265, 10.2307/1422957

Wright, 1985, Memory processing of serial lists by pigeons, monkeys, and people, Science, 229, 287, 10.1126/science.9304205