The Psychometric Properties of the Dot-Probe Paradigm When Used in Pain-Related Attentional Bias Research
Tài liệu tham khảo
Asmundson, 2005, Dot-probe evaluation of selective attentional processing of pain cues in patients with chronic headaches, Pain, 114, 250, 10.1016/j.pain.2004.12.025
Asmundson, 1997, Do patients with chronic pain selectively attend to pain-related information? Preliminary evidence for the mediating role of fear, Pain, 72, 27, 10.1016/S0304-3959(97)00010-9
Asmundson, 2005, Hypervigilance and attentional fixedness in chronic musculoskeletal pain: Consistency of findings across modified stroop and dot-probe tasks, J Pain, 8, 479
Bosson, 2000, Stalking the perfect measure of implicit self-esteem: The blind men and the elephant revisited, J Pers Soc Psych, 79, 631, 10.1037/0022-3514.79.4.631
Boston, 2005, The role of threat-expectancy in acute pain: Effects on attentional bias, coping strategy effectiveness and response to pain, Pain, 119, 168, 10.1016/j.pain.2005.09.032
Bradley, 1998, Attentional bias for threatening facial expressions in anxiety: Manipulation of stimulus duration, Cogn Emot, 12, 737, 10.1080/026999398379411
Brosschot, 1999, Processing bias in anxious subjects and repressors, measured by emotional Stroop interference and attentional allocation, Pers Indiv Dif, 26, 777, 10.1016/S0191-8869(98)00173-1
Dear, 2011, Pain-related attentional biases: The importance of the personal relevance and ecological validity of stimuli, J Pain, 12, 625, 10.1016/j.jpain.2010.11.010
Dehghani, 2003, Selective attention to pain-related information in chronic musculoskeletal pain patients, Pain, 105, 37, 10.1016/S0304-3959(03)00224-0
Dehghani, 2004, Modification of attentional biases in chronic pain patients: A preliminary study, Euro J Pain, 8, 585, 10.1016/j.ejpain.2004.02.003
Eide, 2002, Test-retest reliability of the emotional Stroop task: Examining the paradox of measurement of change, J Psych, 136, 514, 10.1080/00223980209605547
Fazio, 2003, Implicit measures in social cognition: Their meaning and use, Ann Rev Psych, 54, 297, 10.1146/annurev.psych.54.101601.145225
Flor, 1993, Assessment of pain-related cognitions in chronic pain patients, J Psychosom Res, 31, 251, 10.1016/0022-3999(87)90082-1
Haggman, 2010, Attentional biases toward sensory pain words in acute and chronic pain patients, J Pain, 11, 1129, 10.1016/j.jpain.2010.02.017
Keogh, 2001, Selective attentional bias for pain-related stimuli among pain fearful individuals, Pain, 91, 91, 10.1016/S0304-3959(00)00422-X
Khatibi, 2009, Selective attention towards painful faces among chronic pain patients: Evidence from a modified version of the dot-probe, Pain, 142, 42, 10.1016/j.pain.2008.11.020
Korri, 1990, Kinesiophobia: A new view of chronic pain behaviour, Pain Manag, 3, 25
Koster, 2004, Selective attention to threat in the dot probe paradigm: Differentiating vigilance and difficulty to disengage, Behav Res Ther, 42, 1183, 10.1016/j.brat.2003.08.001
Kugler, 1999
Lang, 2005
Liossi, 2009, Time-course of attentional bias for pain-related cues in chronic daily headache sufferers, Euro J Pain, 13, 963, 10.1016/j.ejpain.2008.11.007
Liu, 2006, Repeating the stimulus exposure to investigate what happens after initial selective attention to threatening pictures, Pers Indiv Dif, 40, 1007, 10.1016/j.paid.2005.10.006
Lovibond, 1995
MacLeod, 1986, Attentional Bias in emotional disorders, J Abnorm Psychol, 95, 15, 10.1037/0021-843X.95.1.15
McGowan, 2009, The effect of attentional re-training and threat expectancy in response to acute pain, Pain, 142, 101, 10.1016/j.pain.2008.12.009
McNeil, 1998, Development of the Fear of Pain Questionnaire III, J Behav Med, 21, 389, 10.1023/A:1018782831217
Mogg, 1999, Some methodological issues in assessing attentional biases for threatening faces in anxiety: A replication study using a modified version of the probe detection task, Behav Res Ther, 37, 595, 10.1016/S0005-7967(98)00158-2
Nicholas, 2008, What do the numbers mean? Normative data in chronic pain measures, Pain, 134, 158, 10.1016/j.pain.2007.04.007
Pincus, 2001, Cognitive processing bias in chronic pain: A review and integration, Psychol Bull, 127, 599, 10.1037/0033-2909.127.5.599
Reiss, 1986, Anxiety sensitivity, anxiety frequency and the prediction of fearfulness, Behav Res Ther, 24, 1, 10.1016/0005-7967(86)90143-9
Riemann, 1995, Cognitive processing of personally relevant information, Cogn Emot, 9, 325, 10.1080/02699939508408970
Roelofs, 2005, The role of fear of movement and injury in selective attentional processing in patients with chronic low back pain: A dot-probe evaluation, J Pain, 6, 294, 10.1016/j.jpain.2004.12.011
Roland, 1983, A study of the natural history of back pain. Part I: Development of a reliable and sensitive measure of disability in low-back pain, Spine, 8, 141, 10.1097/00007632-198303000-00004
Schmukle, 2005, Unreliability of the dot probe task, Euro J Pers, 19, 595, 10.1002/per.554
Schoth, 2010, Attentional bias toward pictorial representations of pain in individuals with chronic headache, Clin J Pain, 26, 244, 10.1097/AJP.0b013e3181bed0f9
Sharpe, 2009, Attentional biases in chronic pain associated with rheumatoid arthritis: Hypervigilance or difficulties disengaging?, J Pain, 10, 329, 10.1016/j.jpain.2008.10.005
Siegrist, 1997, Test-retest reliability of different versions of the stroop task, J Psych, 131, 299, 10.1080/00223989709603516
Spielberger, 1983
Staugaard, 2009, Reliability of two versions of the dot-probe task using photographic faces, Psych Sci Quart, 51, 339
Strauss, 2005, Test-retest reliability of standard and emotional Stroop tasks: An investigation of color-word and picture-word versions, Assessment, 12, 330, 10.1177/1073191105276375
Thompson, 2000, Psychometrics is datametrics: The test is not reliable, Edu Psych Measur, 60, 174, 10.1177/0013164400602002