Probabilistic models in human sensorimotor control
Tài liệu tham khảo
Adams, 2004, Experience can change the ‘light-from-above’ prior, Nature Neuroscience, 7, 1057, 10.1038/nn1312
Alais, 2004, The ventriloquist effect results from near-optimal bimodal integration, Current Biology, 14, 257, 10.1016/j.cub.2004.01.029
Barlow, 1987, Human contrast discrimination and the threshold of cortical neurons, Journal of the Optical Society of America A – Optics, Image Science, and Vision, 4, 2366, 10.1364/JOSAA.4.002366
Clamann, 1969, Statistical analysis of motor unit firing patterns in a human skeletal muscle, Biophysical Journal, 9, 1233, 10.1016/S0006-3495(69)86448-9
Cole, 1995
Cox, 1946, Probability, frequency and reasonable expectation, American Journal of Physics, 14, 1, 10.1119/1.1990764
Ernst, 2002, Humans integrate visual and haptic information in a statistically optimal fashion, Nature, 415, 429, 10.1038/415429a
Ernst, 2004, Merging the senses into a robust percept, Trends in Cognitive Science, 8, 162, 10.1016/j.tics.2004.02.002
Flash, 1985, The co-ordination of arm movements: An experimentally confirmed mathematical model, Journal of Neuroscience, 5, 1688, 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.05-07-01688.1985
Frank, 2006, Stochastic order parameter equation of isometric force production revealed by drift-diffusion estimates, Physical Review E. Statistical, Nonlinear, and Soft Matter Physics, 74, 051905, 10.1103/PhysRevE.74.051905
Freedman, 1995, Some issues in the foundation of statistics, Foundations of Science, 1, 19, 10.1007/BF00208723
Geisler, 2002, Illusions, perception and Bayes, Nature Neuroscience, 5, 508, 10.1038/nn0602-508
Glimcher, 2003
Goodwin, 1984
Hamilton, 2004, The scaling of motor noise with muscle strength and motor unit number in humans, Experimental Brain Research, 157, 417, 10.1007/s00221-004-1856-7
Harris, 1998, Signal-dependent noise determines motor planning, Nature, 394, 780, 10.1038/29528
Harris, 2006, The main sequence of saccades optimizes speed-accuracy trade-off, Biological Cybernetics, 95, 21, 10.1007/s00422-006-0064-x
Haruno, 2005, Optimal control of redundant muscles in step-tracking wrist movements, Journal of Neurophysiology, 94, 4244, 10.1152/jn.00404.2005
von Helmholtz, 1867
Hillis, 2004, Slant from texture and disparity cues: Optimal cue combination, Journal of Vision, 4, 967, 10.1167/4.12.1
Hogan, 1984, An organizing principle for a class of voluntary movements, Journal of Neuroscience, 4, 2745, 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.04-11-02745.1984
Jacobs, 1999, Optimal integration of texture and motion cues to depth, Vision Research, 39, 3621, 10.1016/S0042-6989(99)00088-7
Jaynes, E. (1995). Probability theory: The logic of science.
Jones, 2002, Sources of signal-dependent noise during isometric force production, Journal of Neurophysiology, 88, 1533, 10.1152/jn.2002.88.3.1533
Knill, 2003, Do humans optimally integrate stereo and texture information for judgments of surface slant?, Vision Research, 43, 2539, 10.1016/S0042-6989(03)00458-9
Kording, 2004, Bayesian integration in force estimation, Journal of Neurophysiology, 92, 3161, 10.1152/jn.00275.2004
Kording, 2004, Bayesian integration in sensorimotor learning, Nature, 427, 244, 10.1038/nature02169
Kording, 2004, The loss function of sensorimotor learning, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA, 101, 9839, 10.1073/pnas.0308394101
Kuo, 1995, An optimal-control model for analyzing human postural balance, IEEE Transactions on Biomedical Engineering, 42, 87, 10.1109/10.362914
MacKay, 2003
Matthews, 1996, Relationship of firing intervals of human motor units to the trajectory of post-spike after-hyperpolarization and synaptic noise, Journal of Physiology, 492, 597, 10.1113/jphysiol.1996.sp021332
Merfeld, 1999, Humans use internal model to estimate gravity and linear acceleration, Nature, 398, 615, 10.1038/19303
Miyazaki, 2005, Testing Bayesian models of human coincidence timing, Journal of Neurophysiology, 94, 395, 10.1152/jn.01168.2004
Morasso, 1981, Spatial control of arm movements, Experimental Brain Research, 42, 223, 10.1007/BF00236911
Pandy, 2001, Computer modeling and simulation of human movement, Annual Review of Biomedical Engineering, 3, 245, 10.1146/annurev.bioeng.3.1.245
Poulton, 1979, Models for biases in judging sensory magnitude, Psychological Bulletin, 86, 777, 10.1037/0033-2909.86.4.777
Ramachandran, 1988, Perceiving shape from shading, Scientific American, 259, 76, 10.1038/scientificamerican0888-76
Schmidt, 1979, Motor-output variability: A theory for the accuracy of rapid motor acts, Psychological Review, 47, 415, 10.1037/0033-295X.86.5.415
Schmidt, 1978, Sources of inaccuracy in rapid movements, 183
Scott, 2004, Optimal feedback control and the neural basis of volitional motor control, Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 5, 532, 10.1038/nrn1427
Scott, 1994, The computation of position sense from spindles in mono- and multiarticular muscles, Journal of Neuroscience, 14, 7529, 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.14-12-07529.1994
Smeets, 2006, Sensory integration does not lead to sensory calibration, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA, 103, 18781, 10.1073/pnas.0607687103
Sober, 2005, Flexible strategies for sensory integration during motor planning, Nature Neuroscience, 8, 490, 10.1038/nn1427
Soechting, 1981, Invariant characteristics of a pointing movement in man, Journal of Neuroscience, 1, 710, 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.01-07-00710.1981
Srinivasan, 2006, Computer optimization of a minimal biped model discovers walking and running, Nature, 439, 72, 10.1038/nature04113
Tassinari, 2006, Combining priors and noisy visual cues in a rapid pointing task, Journal of Neuroscience, 26, 10154, 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2779-06.2006
Todorov, 2004, Optimality principles in sensorimotor control, Nature Neuroscience, 7, 907, 10.1038/nn1309
Todorov, 2005, Stochastic optimal control and estimation methods adapted to the noise characteristics of the sensorimotor system, Neural Computation, 17, 1084, 10.1162/0899766053491887
Trommershauser, 2003, Statistical decision theory and the selection of rapid, goal-directed movements, Journal of the Optical Society of America A – Optics, Image Science, and Vision, 20, 1419, 10.1364/JOSAA.20.001419
Uno, 1989, Formation and control of optimal trajectories in human multijoint arm movements: Minimum torque-change model, Biological Cybernetics, 61, 89, 10.1007/BF00204593
Van Beers, 1996, How humans combine simultaneous proprioceptive and visual position information, Experimental Brain Research, 111, 253, 10.1007/BF00227302
Van Beers, 1999, Integration of proprioceptive and visual position-information: An experimentally supported model, Journal of Neurophysiology, 81, 1355, 10.1152/jn.1999.81.3.1355
Van Beers, 2002, When feeling is more important than seeing in sensorimotor adaptation, Current Biology, 12, 834, 10.1016/S0960-9822(02)00836-9
Viviani, 1995, Minimum-jerk model, two-thirds power law and isochrony: Converging approaches to the study of movement planning, Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 21, 32, 10.1037/0096-1523.21.1.32
Weiss, 2002, Motion illusions as optimal percepts, Nature Neuroscience, 5, 598, 10.1038/nn0602-858
Wolpert, 1995, An internal model for sensorimotor integration, Science, 269, 1880, 10.1126/science.7569931
Yuille, 2006, Vision as Bayesian inference: Analysis by synthesis?, Trends in Cognitive Science, 10.1016/j.tics.2006.05.002