Distance perception mediated through nested contact relations among surfaces

Perception & Psychophysics - Tập 63 - Trang 1-15 - 2001
Jeanette C. Meng1, H. A. Sedgwick1
1Vision Science Department, State University of New York, State College of Optometry, New York

Tóm tắt

In complex natural scenes, objects at different spatial locations can usually be related to each other through nested contact relations among adjoining surfaces. Our research asks how well human observers, under monocular static viewing conditions, are able to utilize this information in distance perception. We present computer-generated naturalistic scenes of a cube resting on a platform, which is in turn resting on the ground. Observers adjust the location of a marker on the ground to equal the perceived distance of the cube. We find that (1) perceived distance of the cube varies appropriately as the perceived location of contact between the platform and the ground varies; (2) variability increases systematically as the relating surfaces move apart; and (3) certain local edge alignments allow precise propagation of distance information. These results demonstrate considerable efficiency in the mediation of distance perception through nested contact relations among surfaces.

Tài liệu tham khảo

Alhazen, I. (1989). Book of optics. InThe optics of Ibn-Haytham (Vol. 1, A. I. Sabra, Trans.). London: University of London, Warburg Institute. (Alhazen lived approximately 965–1040 C.E.; when he wrote this work is not known.) Becker, W., &Fuchs, A. F. (1969). Further properties of the human saccadic system: Eye movements and correction saccades with and without visual fixation points.Vision Research,9, 1247–1258. Brooks, A., &Stevens, K. A. (1989). Binocular depth from surfaces versus volumes.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance,15, 479–484. Carpenter, R. H. S. (1988).Movements of the eyes (2nd ed.). London: Pion. Cutting, J. E., &Vishton, P. M. (1995). Perceiving layout and knowing distances: The integration, relative potency, and contextual use of different information about depth. In W. Epstein & S. Rogers (Eds.),Perception of space and motion (pp. 69–117). New York: Academic Press. Gibson, E. J., &Bergman, R. (1954). The effect of training on absolute estimation of distance over the ground.Journal of Experimental Psychology,48, 473–482. Gibson, E. J., Bergman, R., &Purdy, J. (1955). The effect of prior training with a scale of distance on absolute and relative judgments of distance over ground.Journal of Experimental Psychology,50, 97–105. Gibson, J. J. (1950).The perception of the visual world. Boston: Houghton Mifflin. Gillam, B. (1995). The perception of spatial layout from static optical information. In W. Epstein & S. Rogers (Eds.),Perception of space and motion (pp. 23–67). New York: Academic Press. Gillam, B., &Sedgwick, H. A. (1996). The interaction of stereopsis and perspective in the perception of depth.Perception,25 (Suppl.), 70. Glennerster, A., &McKee, S. P. (1999). Bias and sensitivity of stereo judgments in the presence of a slanted reference plane.Vision Research,39, 3057–3069. He, Z. J., &Nakayama, K. (1992). Surfaces versus features in visual search.Nature,359, 231–233. He, Z. J., &Nakayama, K. (1994). Apparent motion determined by surface layout not by disparity or three-dimensional distance.Nature,367, 173–175. He, Z. J., &Nakayama, K. (1995). Visual attention to surfaces in three-dimensional space.Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences,92, 11155–11159. Kanade, T. (1980). A theory of origami world.Artificial Intelligence,13, 279–311. Kersten, D., Mamassian, P., &Knill, D. C. (1997). Moving cast shadows induce apparent motion in depth.Perception,26, 171–192. Komoda, M. K., Festinger, L., Phillips, L. J., Duckman, R. H., &Young, R. A. (1973). Some observations concerning saccadic eye movements.Vision Research,13, 1009–1020. Komoda, M. K., Festinger, L., &Sherry, J. (1977). The accuracy of two-dimensional saccades in the absence of continuing retinal stimulation.Vision Research,17, 1231–1232. Levin, C. A., &Haber, R. N. (1993). Visual angle as a determinant of perceived interobject distance.Perception & Psychophysics,54, 250–259. Loomis, J. M., Da Silva, J. A., Fujita, N., &Fukusima, S. S. (1992). Visual space perception and visually directed action.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance,18, 906–921. Loomis, J. M., Da Silva, J. A., Philbeck, J. W., &Fukusima, S. S. (1996). Visual perception of location and distance.Current Directions in Psychological Science,5, 72–77. Loomis, J. M., &Philbeck, J. W. (1999). Is the anisotropy of perceived 3-D shape invariant across scale?Perception & Psychophysics,61, 397–402. Mackworth, J. F., &Mackworth, N. H. (1958). Eye fixations recorded on changing visual scenes by the television eye-marker.Journal of the Optical Society of America,48, 439–445. Madison, C. M., &Kersten, D. J. (1999). Use of interreflection and shadow for surface contact.Investigative Ophthalmology & Visual Science,40 (Suppl. 4), s748. Mitchison, G. J., &Westheimer, G. (1984). The perception of depth in simple figures.Vision Research,24, 1063–1073. Nakayama, K., He, Z. J., &Shimojo, S. (1995). Visual surface representation: A critical link between lower-level and higher-level vision. In S. M. Kosslyn & D. N. Osherson (Eds.),Visual cognition: An invitation to cognitive science (2nd ed., Vol. 2, pp. 1–71). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Nakayama, K., &Shimojo, S. (1992). Experiencing and perceiving visual surfaces.Science,257, 1357–1363. Sedgwick, H. A. (1983). Environmental-centered representation of spatial layout: Available visual information from texture and perspective. In J. Beck, B. Hope, & A. Rosenfeld (Eds.),Human and machine vision (pp. 425–458). San Diego: Academic Press. Sedgwick, H. A. (1986). Space perception. In K. R. Bosaff, L. Kaufman, & J. P. Thomas (Eds.),Handbook of perception and human performance: Sensory processes and perception (Vol. 1, pp. 21.1–21.57). New York: Wiley. Sedgwick, H. A. (1987a). Layout2: A production system modeling visual perspective information. InProceedings of the First International Conference on Computer Vision (pp. 662–666). Washington, DC: IEEE Computer Society Press. Sedgwick, H. A. (1987b).A production system modeling high-level visual perspective information for spatial layout (Tech. Rep. No. 298). New York: New York University, Department of Computer Science. Sedgwick, H. A. (1989). Combining multiple forms of visual information to specify contact relations in spatial layout. In P. S. Schenker (Ed.),Sensor fusion II: Human and machine strategies (SPIE Proceedings, Vol. 1198, pp. 447–458). Bellingham, WA: SPIE. Sedgwick, H. A. (in press). Visual space perception. In E. B. Goldstein (Ed.),Handbook of perception. Blackwell. Sedgwick, H. A., &Levy, S. (1985). Environment-centered and viewer-centered perception of surface orientation.Computer Vision, Graphics, & Image Processing,31, 248–260. Sinai, M. J., Ooi, T. L., &He, Z. J. (1998). Terrain influences the accurate judgement of distance.Nature,395, 497–500. Sugihara, K. (1984). An algebraic approach to shape-from-image problems.Artificial Intelligence,23, 59–95. Thomson, J. A. (1983). Is continuous visual monitoring necessary in visually guided locomotion?Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance,9, 427–443. Toye, R. C. (1986). The effect of viewing position on the perceived layout of space.Perception & Psychophysics,40, 85–92. Yarbus, A. L. (1967).Eye movements and vision (B. Haigh, Trans.). New York: Plenum. Yonas, A., Goldsmith, L. T., &Hallstrom, J. L. (1978). Development of sensitivity to information provided by cast shadows in pictures.Perception,7, 333–341.