Attentional spread in the statistical processing of visual displays

Perception & Psychophysics - Tập 67 Số 1 - Trang 1-13 - 2005
Sang Chul Chong1, Anne Treisman
1Princeton University, Princeton, New Jersey, USA. [email protected]

Tóm tắt

Từ khóa


Tài liệu tham khảo

Ariely, D. (2001). Seeing sets: Representation by statistical properties.Psychological Science,12, 157–162.

Ariely, D., & Burbeck, C. A. (1995). Statistical encoding of multiple stimuli: A theory of distributed representation [Abstract].Investigative Ophthalmology & Visual Science,36(Suppl.), No. 8472.

Austin, E., &Enns, J. T. (2000). Change detection: Paying attention to detail.PSYCHE,6, 1–16.

Brainard, D. H. (1997). The Psychophysics Toolbox.Spatial Vision,10, 433–436.

Chong, S. C., &Treisman, A. (2003). Representation of statistical properties.Vision Research,43, 393–404.

Delis, D. C., Robertson, L. C., &Efron, R. (1986). Hemispheric specialization of memory for visual hierarchical stimuli.Neuropsychologia,24, 205–214.

Desimone, R., &Duncan, J. (1995). Neural mechanisms of selective visual attention.Annual Review of Neuroscience,18, 193–222.

Duncan, J. (1984). Selective attention and the organization of visual information.Journal of Experimental Psychology: General,113, 501–517.

Farell, B., &Pelli, D. G. (1993). Can we attend to large and small at the same time?Vision Research,33, 2757–2772.

Field, D. J. (1987). Relations between the statistics of natural images and the response properties of cortical cells.Journal of the Optical Society of America A,4, 2379–2394.

Fink, G. R., Halligan, P. W., Marshall, J. C., Frith, C. D., Frackowiak, R. S. J., &Dolan, R. J. (1996). Where in the brain does visual attention select the forest and the trees?Nature,382, 626–628.

Geisler, W. S., Perry, J. S., Super, B. J., &Gallogly, D. P. (2001). Edge co-occurrence in natural images predicts contour grouping performance.Vision Research,41, 711–724.

Haralick, R. M., Shanmugam, K., &Dinstein, I. (1973). Textural features for image classification.IEEE Transactions on Systems, Man, & Cybernetics,3, 610–621.

Heinze, H. J., Hinrichs, H., Scholz, M., Burchert, W., &Mangun, G. R. (1998). Neural mechanisms of global and local processing: A combined PET and ERP study.Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience,10, 485–498.

Helson, H. (1947). Adaptation-level as frame of reference for prediction of psychophysical data.American Journal of Psychology,60, 1–29.

Hock, H. S., Park, C. L., &Schöner, G. (2002). Self-organized pattern formation: Experimental dissection of motion detection and motion integration by variation of attentional spread.Vision Research,42, 991–1003.

Hoffman, J. E. (1980). Interaction between global and local levels of a form.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance,6, 222–234.

Julesz, B. (1981). Textons, the elements of texture perception, and their interactions.Nature,290, 91–97.

Kastner, S., De Weerd, P., Desimone, R., &Ungerleider, L. G. (1998). Mechanisms of directed attention in the human extrastriate cortex as revealed by functional MRI.Science,282, 108–111.

Miller, J. (1981). Global precedence in attention and decision.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance,7, 1161–1174.

Navon, D. (1977). Forest before trees: The precedence of global features in visual perception.Cognitive Psychology,9, 353–383.

Pomerantz, J. R. (1983). Global and local precedence: Selective attention in form and motion perception.Journal of Experimental Psychology: General,112, 516–540.

Posner, M. I., &Keele, S. W. (1968). On the genesis of abstract ideas.Journal of Experimental Psychology,77, 353–363.

Rensink, R. A. (2002). Change detection.Annual Review of Psychology,53, 245–277.

Reynolds, J., Chelazzi, L., &Desimone, R. (1999). Competitive mechanisms subserve attention in macaque areas V2 and V4.Journal of Neuroscience,19, 1736–1753.

Robertson, L. C., Egly, R., Lamb, M. R., &Kerth, L. (1993). Spatial attention and cuing to global and local levels of hierarchical structure.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance,19, 471–487.

Simons, D. J., &Levin, D. T. (1997). Change blindness.Trends in Cognitive Sciences,1, 261–267.

Teghtsoonian, M. (1965). The judgment of size.American Journal of Psychology,78, 392–402.

Treisman, A. (1969). Strategies and models of selective attention.Psychological Review,76, 282–299.

Treisman, A. (1991). Search, similarity, and integration of features between and within dimensions.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance,17, 652–676.

Treisman, A., &Gormican, S. (1988). Feature analysis in early vision: Evidence from search asymmetries.Psychological Review,95, 15–48.

Treisman, A., &Souther, J. (1985). Search asymmetry: A diagnostic for preattentive processing of separable features.Journal of Experimental Psychology: General,114, 285–310.

Ward, L. M. (1982). Determinants of attention to local and global features of visual forms.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance,8, 562–581.

Watamaniuk, S. N. J., &McKee, S. P. (1998). Simultaneous encoding of direction at a local and global scale.Perception & Psychophysics,60, 191–200.

Watamaniuk, S. N. J., &Sekuler, R. (1992). Temporal and spatial integration in dynamic random-dot stimuli.Vision Research,32, 2341–2347.

Woods, R. L., &Thomson, W. D. (1993). A comparison of psychometric methods for measuring the contrast sensitivity of experienced observers.Clinical Vision Science,8, 401–415.