Animacy and competition in relative clause production: A cross-linguistic investigation
Tài liệu tham khảo
Abasolo, 1999, Cláusulas relativas con conjunción en las lenguas románicas, eslavas y vasca, Revista de Filología Románica, 16, 267
Acheson, 2011, The rhymes that the reader perused confused the meaning: Phonological effects during on-line sentence comprehension, Journal of Memory and Language, 65, 193, 10.1016/j.jml.2011.04.006
Arnold, 2007, The effect of additional characters on choice of referring expression: Everyone counts, Journal of Memory and Language, 56, 521, 10.1016/j.jml.2006.09.007
Baird, 1974, Recall of grammatical relations within clause-containing sentences, Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 3, 165, 10.1007/BF01067574
Bello, 1984
Berndt, 1996, Comprehension of reversible sentences in “agrammatism” a meta-analysis, Cognition, 58, 289, 10.1016/0010-0277(95)00682-6
Bock, 1987, An effect of the accessibility of word forms on sentence structure, Journal of Memory and Language, 26, 119, 10.1016/0749-596X(87)90120-3
Bock, 2007, Persistent structural priming from language comprehension to language production, Cognition, 104, 437, 10.1016/j.cognition.2006.07.003
Bock, 1980, Syntactic effects of information availability in sentence production, Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 19, 467, 10.1016/S0022-5371(80)90321-7
Bock, 1994, Language production: Grammatical encoding
Bock, 1992, From conceptual roles to structural relations: Bridging the syntactic cleft, Psychological Review, 99, 150, 10.1037/0033-295X.99.1.150
Bock, 1985, Conceptual accessibility and syntactic structure in sentence formulation, Cognition, 21, 47, 10.1016/0010-0277(85)90023-X
Branigan, 2008, Contributions of animacy to grammatical function assignment and word order during production, Lingua, 118, 10.1016/j.lingua.2007.02.003
Caramazza, 1991, Selective impairment of thematic role assignment in sentence processing, Brain and Language, 41, 402, 10.1016/0093-934X(91)90164-V
Casielles Suárez, 1996, ¿Es la interpretación arbitraria realmente arbitraria?, Revista Española de Lingüística, 26, 359
Chang, 2006, Becoming Syntactic, Psychological Review, 113, 234, 10.1037/0033-295X.113.2.234
Chierchia, 1990
Clark, 1965, Some structural properties of simple active and passive sentences, Journal of Verbal Learning & Verbal Behavior, 4, 365, 10.1016/S0022-5371(65)80073-1
Cohen, 1993, Psyscope: A new graphic interactive environment for designing psychological experiments, Behavioral Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers, 25, 257, 10.3758/BF03204507
Costa, 2005, On the categorical nature of the semantic interference effect in the picture-word interference paradigm, Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 12, 125, 10.3758/BF03196357
Crain, 1985, On not being led up the garden path; the use of context by the psychological parser, 320
De Smedt, 1987, Incremental sentence production, self-correction and coordination, 365
Dell, 1999, Connectionist models of language production: Lexical access and grammatical encoding, Cognitive Science. A Multidisciplinary Journal, 23, 517, 10.1207/s15516709cog2304_6
Dell, G. S., Chang, F., & Griffin, Z. M. (2001). In H. Morten (Ed.), Connectionist models of language production: Lexical access and grammatical encoding, Christiansen.
Dell, 2008, Saying the right word at the right time: Syntagmatic and paradigmatic interference in sentence production, Language and Cognitive Processes, 23, 583, 10.1080/01690960801920735
Dell, 1992, Stages of lexical access in language production, Cognition, 42, 287, 10.1016/0010-0277(92)90046-K
Dell, 1994, Inhibition in interactive activation models of linguistic selection and sequencing, Vol. xiv, 409
Ferreira, 1996, Is it better to give than to donate? Syntactic flexibility in language production, Journal of Memory and Language, 35, 724, 10.1006/jmla.1996.0038
Ferreira, 2003, The persistence of optional complementizer production: Why saying “that” is not saying “that” at all, Journal of Memory and Language, 48, 379, 10.1016/S0749-596X(02)00523-5
Ferreira, 2000, Effect of ambiguity and lexical availability on syntactic and lexical production, Cognitive Psychology, 40, 296, 10.1006/cogp.1999.0730
Ferreira, 2002, Proactive interference effects on sentence production, Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 9, 795, 10.3758/BF03196337
Ferreira, 2002, How incremental is language production? Evidence from the production of utterances requiring the computation of arithmetic sums, Journal of Memory and Language, 46, 57, 10.1006/jmla.2001.2797
Ferretti, 2006, The recovery of thematic role structure during noun-noun interpretation, Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 13, 423, 10.3758/BF03193864
Ferretti, 2001, Integrating verbs, situation schemas, and thematic role, Journal of Memory & Language, 44, 516, 10.1006/jmla.2000.2728
Fox, 1990, A discourse explanation of the grammar of relative clauses in English conversation, Language, 66, 297, 10.2307/414888
Fukumura, 2011, How does similarity-based interference affect the choice of referring expression?, Journal of Memory and Language, 65, 331, 10.1016/j.jml.2011.06.001
Garrett, 1980, Levels of processing in sentence production, 177
Gennari, 2008, Semantic indeterminacy in object relative clauses, Journal of Memory & Language, 58, 161, 10.1016/j.jml.2007.07.004
Gennari, 2009, Linking production and comprehension processes: The case of relative clauses, Cognition, 111, 1, 10.1016/j.cognition.2008.12.006
Glaser, 1984, The time course of picture-word interference, Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 10
Glaser, 1989, Context effects in stroop-like word and picture processing, Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 118
Gordon, 2001, Memory interference during language processing, Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 27, 1411, 10.1037/0278-7393.27.6.1411
Gordon, 2004, Effects of noun phrase type on sentence complexity, Journal of Memory and Language, 51, 97, 10.1016/j.jml.2004.02.003
Hamburger, 1982, Relative acquisition, Vol. II
Hartsuiker, 2003, Morphophonological influences on the construction of subject–verb agreement, Memory and Cognition, 31, 1316, 10.3758/BF03195814
Haskell, 2005, Constituent structure and linear order in language production: Evidence from subject–verb agreement, Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 31, 891, 10.1037/0278-7393.31.5.891
Jaeger, 2010, Redundancy and reduction: Speakers manage syntactic information density, Cognitive Psychology, 61, 23, 10.1016/j.cogpsych.2010.02.002
Jaeggli, 1986, Arbitrary plural pronominals, Natural Language and Linguistic Theory, 4, 43, 10.1007/BF00136264
Kempen, 1987, An incremental procedural grammar for sentence formulation, Cognitive Science. A Multidisciplinary Journal, 11, 201, 10.1207/s15516709cog1102_5
King, 1991, Individual differences in syntactic processing: The role of working memory, Journal of Memory and Language, 30, 580, 10.1016/0749-596X(91)90027-H
Konopka, 2012, Planning ahead: How recent experience with structures and words changes the scope of linguistic planning, Journal of Memory and Language, 66, 143, 10.1016/j.jml.2011.08.003
Kostić, 1986
Levelt, 1989
Lewis, 2005, An activation-based model of sentence processing as skilled memory retrieval, Cognitive Science. A Multidisciplinary Journal, 29, 375, 10.1207/s15516709cog0000_25
MacDonald, 1999, Distributional information in language comprehension, production, and acquisition: Three puzzles and a moral
MacWhinney, 1988, The processing of restrictive relative clauses in Hungarian, Cognition, 29, 95, 10.1016/0010-0277(88)90034-0
Mak, 2002, The influence of animacy on relative clause processing, Journal of Memory and Language, 47, 50, 10.1006/jmla.2001.2837
McDonald, 1993, Word and world order: Semantics, phonological, and metrical determinants of serial position, Cognitive Psychology, 25, 188, 10.1006/cogp.1993.1005
McRae, K., Hare, M., Ferretti, T., & Elman, J. (2001). Activating verbs from typical agents, patients, instruments, and locations via event schemas. In: Paper presented at the annual conference of the Cognitive Science Society. University of Edimburgh.
McRae, 1997, Thematic roles as verb-specific concepts, Language and Cognitive Processes, 12, 137, 10.1080/016909697386835
Meyer, 1996, Lexical access in phrase and sentence production: Results from picture–word interference experiments, Journal of Memory and Language, 35, 477, 10.1006/jmla.1996.0026
Montag, J. L., & MacDonald, M. C. (2011). How visual salience affects structure choice. Implications for audience design. Poster presented at the 24th Annual CUNY Conference on Human Sentence Processing, Stanford, CA.
Mrazović, 1991
Myachykov, 2008, Perceptual priming and structural choice in Russian sentence production, Journal of Cognitive Science, 6, 31, 10.17791/jcs.2008.9.1.31
Pickering, 2007, Do people use language production to make predictions during comprehension?, Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 11, 105, 10.1016/j.tics.2006.12.002
Prat-Sala, 2000, Discourse constraints on syntactic processing in language production: A cross-linguistic study in English and Spanish, Journal of Memory and Language, 42, 168, 10.1006/jmla.1999.2668
Race, 2003, The use of “that” in the production and comprehension of object relative clauses, 946
Roland, 2007, Frequency of basic English grammatical structures: A corpus analysis, Journal of Memory and Language, 57, 348, 10.1016/j.jml.2007.03.002
Saffran, 1998, Semantic influences on thematic role assignment: Evidence from normals and aphasics, Brain & Language, 62, 255, 10.1006/brln.1997.1918
Schriefers, 1990, Exploring the time course of lexical access in language production: Picture–word interference studies, Journal of Memory and Language, 29, 86, 10.1016/0749-596X(90)90011-N
Sheldon, 1974, The role of parallel function in the acquisition of relative clauses in English, Journal of Verbal Learning & Verbal Behavior, 13, 272, 10.1016/S0022-5371(74)80064-2
Slevc, 2011, Saying what’s on your mind: Working memory effects on sentence production, Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 37, 1503, 10.1037/a0024350
Slobin, 1982, Children use canonical sentence schemas: A crosslinguistic study of word order and inflections, Cognition, 12, 229, 10.1016/0010-0277(82)90033-6
Smith, 1999, High level processing scope in spoken sentence production, Cognition, 73, 205, 10.1016/S0010-0277(99)00053-0
Smith, 2004, Horizontal information flow in spoken sentence production, Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 30, 675, 10.1037/0278-7393.30.3.675
2005
Tanaka, 2011, Conceptual influences on word order and voice in sentence production: Evidence from Japanese, Journal of Memory and Language, 65, 318, 10.1016/j.jml.2011.04.009
Traxler, 2002, Processing subject and object relative clauses: Evidence from eye movements, Journal of Memory and Language, 47, 69, 10.1006/jmla.2001.2836
Trueswell, 1994, Semantic influences on parsing: Use of thematic role information in syntactic ambiguity resolution, Journal of Memory and Language, 33, 285, 10.1006/jmla.1994.1014
Turner, 1967, The acquisition of sentence voice and reversibility, Child Development, 38, 649, 10.2307/1127243
van der Lely, 1990, Comprehension of reversible sentences in specifically language-impaired children, Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, 55, 101, 10.1044/jshd.5501.101
Van Dyke, 2007, Interference effects from grammatically unavailable constituents during sentence processing, Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 33, 330, 10.1037/0278-7393.33.2.407
Van Dyke, 2003, Distinguishing effects of structure and decay on attachment and repair: A cue-based parsing account of recovery from misanalyzed ambiguities, Journal of Memory and Language, 49, 285, 10.1016/S0749-596X(03)00081-0
Van Dyke, 2006, Retrieval interference in sentence comprehension, Journal of Memory and Language, 55, 157, 10.1016/j.jml.2006.03.007
Van Dyke, 2011, Cue-dependent interference in comprehension, Journal of Memory and Language, 65, 247, 10.1016/j.jml.2011.05.002
Vigliocco, 2002, The interplay of meaning, sound, and syntax in sentence production, Psychological Bulletin, 128, 442, 10.1037/0033-2909.128.3.442
Wagner, 2010, On the flexibility of grammatical advance planning during sentence production: Effects of cognitive load on multiple lexical access, Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 36, 423, 10.1037/a0018619
Weiner, 1983, Constraints on agentless passive, Journal of Linguistics, 19, 29, 10.1017/S0022226700007441
Wells, 2009, Experience and sentence processing: Statistical learning and relative clause comprehension, Cognitive Psychology, 58, 250, 10.1016/j.cogpsych.2008.08.002
Yamashita, 2001, “Long before short” preference in the production of a head-final language, Cognition, 81, B45, 10.1016/S0010-0277(01)00121-4
Zukowsky, 2009, Elicited production of relative clauses in children with Williams syndrome, Language and Cognitive Processes, 24, 1, 10.1080/01690960801966118