The effects of information framing on the practices of physicians

Journal of General Internal Medicine - Tập 14 - Trang 633-642 - 1999
Patricia McGettigan1, Ketrina Sly1, Dianne O’Connell1, Suzanne Hill1, David Henry1
1the Disciplines of Clinical Pharmacology and Psychiatry, Faculty of Medicine and Health Sciences, The University of Newcastle, Australia

Tóm tắt

OBJECTIVE: The presentation format of clinical trial results, or the “frame,” may influence perceptions about the worth of a treatment. The extent and consistency of that influence are unclear. We undertook a systematic review of the published literature on the effects of information framing on the practices of physicians. DESIGN: Relevant articles were retrieved using bibliographic and electronic searches. Information was extracted from each in relation to study design, frame type, parameter assessed, assessment scale, clinical setting, intervention, results, and factors modifying the frame effect. MAIN RESULTS: Twelve articles reported randomized trials investigating the effect of framing on doctors’ opinions or intended practices. Methodological shortcomings were numerous. Seven papers investigated the effect of presenting clinical trial results in terms of relative risk reduction, or absolute risk reductions or the number needing to be treated; gain/loss (positive/negative) terms were used in four papers; verbal/numeric terms in one. In simple clinical scenarios, results expressed in relative risk reduction or gain terms were viewed most positively by doctors. Factors that reduced the impact of framing included the risk of causing harm, preexisting prejudices about treatments, the type of decision, the therapeutic yield, clinical experience, and costs. No study investigated the effect of framing on actual clinical practice. CONCLUSIONS: While a framing effect may exist, particularly when results are presented in terms of proportional or absolute measures of gain or loss, it appears highly susceptible to modification, and even neutralization, by other factors that influence doctors’ decision making. Its effects on actual clinical practice are unknown.

Tài liệu tham khảo

Tversky A, Kahnemann D. The framing of decisions and the psychology of choice. Science. 1981;211:453–8. Kahnemann D, Tversky A. Choices, values and frames. Am Psychologist. 1984;39:341–50. Forrow L, Taylor WC, Arnold RM. Absolutely relative: how research results are summarized can affect treatment decisions. Am J Med. 1992;92:121–4. Naylor CD, Chen E, Strauss B. Measured enthusiasm: does the method of reporting trial results alter perceptions of therapeutic effectiveness? Ann Intern Med. 1992;117:916–21. Malenka DJ, Baron JA, Johansen S, Wahrenberger JW, Ross JM. The framing effect of relative and absolute risk. J Gen Intern Med. 1993;8:543–8. Bucher HC, Weinbacher M, Gyr K. Influence of method of reporting study results on decision of physicians to prescribe drugs to lower cholesterol concentration. BMJ. 1994;309:761. Bobbio M, Demichelis B, Guido G. Completeness of reporting trial results: effect on physicians’ willingness to prescribe. Lancet. 1994;343(8907):1209–11. Cranney M, Walley T. Same information, different decisions: the influence of evidence on the management of hypertension in the elderly. Br J Gen Pract. 1996;46:661–3. Nikolajevic-Sarunac J, Henry DA, O’Connell DL, Robertson J. Effects of information framing on the intentions of family physicians to prescribe long-term hormone replacement therapy. J Gen Intern Med. 1999;14:591–98. Ward JE, Shah S, Donnelly N. Resource allocation in cardiac rehabilitation: MuirGray’s aphorisms might apply in Australia. Clinician in Management. 1999;8:24–6. McNeil BJ, Pauker SG, Sox HC, Tversky A. On the elicitation of preferences for alternative therapies. N Engl J Med. 1982;306:1259–62. Marteau TM. Framing of information: its influence upon decisions of doctors and patients. Br J Soc Psychol. 1989;28:89–94. Christensen C, Heckerling PS, Mackesy ME, Bernstein LM, Elstein AS. Framing bias among expert and novice physicians. Acad Med. 1991;6619:576–8. Hux JE, Levinton CM, Naylor CD. Prescribing propensity: influence of life-expectancy gains and drug costs. J Gen Intern Med. 1994;9:195–201. Timmermans D. The roles of experience and domain of expertise in using numerical and verbal probability terms in medical decisions. Med Decis Making. 1994;14:146–56. Hypertension Detection and Follow-up Program Cooperative Group. Five-year findings of the Hypertension Detection and Follow-up Program, 1: reduction in mortality of persons with high blood pressure, including mild hypertension. JAMA. 1979;242:2562–71. The Lipid Research Clinics Program. The Lipid Research Clinics Primary Prevention Trial results, 1: reduction in incidence of coronary disease. JAMA. 1984;251:351–64. Frick MH, Elo O, Haapa K, et al. Helsinki Heart Study: primary prevention trial with gemfibrizol in middle-aged men with dyslipidaemia: safety of treatment, changes in risk factors, and incidence of coronary heart disease. N Engl J Med. 1987;317:1237–45. MRC Working Party. MRC trial of treatment of hypertension in older adults: principal results. BMJ. 1992;304:405–11. Oldridge NB, Guyatt GH, Fiscer ME, Rimm AA. Cardiac rehabilitation after myocardial infarction: combined experience of randomized controlled trials. JAMA. 1988;260:945–50. Skolbekken JA. Communicating the risk reduction achieved by cholesterol reducing drugs. BMJ. 1998;316:1956–8. Kahnemann D, Tversky A. Prospect theory: an analysis of decision under risk. Econometrica. 1979;47:263–91. Schneider SL. Framing and conflict: aspiration level contingency, the status quo, and the current theories of risky choice. J Exp Psychol. 1992;18(5):1040–57. Jou J, Shanteau J, Jackson Harris R. An information processing view of framing effects: the role of causal schemas in decision making. Memory Cognition. 1996;24(1):1–15. Steiner JF. Talking about treatment: the language of populations and the language of individuals. Ann Intern Med. 1999;130:618–22. Lamas GA, Pfeffer MA, Hamm P, Wertheimer J, Roleau JL, Braunwald E, for the SAVE Investigators. Do the results of randomized clinical trials of cardiovascular drugs influence medical practice? N Engl J Med. 1992;327:241–7. Kassirer JP. Incorporating patients’ preferences into medical decisions. N Engl J Med. 1994;330:1895–6. Berwick DM, Fineberg HV, Weinstein MC. When doctors meet numbers. Am J Med. 1981;71:991–8. Gigerenzer G. The psychology of good judgement: frequency formats and simple algorithms. Med Decis Making. 1996;16:273–80. Mazur DJ, Hickam DH. Treatment preferences of patients and physicians: influences of summary data when framing effects are controlled. Med Decis Making. 1990;10:2–5. Redelmeier DA, Shafir E. Medical decision making in situations that offer multiple alternatives. JAMA. 1995;273(4):302–5. Ayanian JZ, Hauptman PJ, Guadagnoli E, Antman EM, Pashos CL, McNeil BJ. Knowledge and practices of generalist and specialist physicians regarding drug therapy for acute myocardial infarction. N Engl J Med. 1994;331(17):1136–42. Freidman PD, Brett AS, Mayo-Smith MF. Differences in generalists’ and cardiologists’ perceptions of cardiovascular risk and the outcomes of preventive therapy in cardiovascular disease. Ann Intern Med. 1996;124:414–21. Woo B, Woo B, Cook F, Weisberg M, Goldman L. Screening procedures in the asymptomatic adult: comparison of physicians’ recommendations, patients’ desires, published guidelines, and actual practice. JAMA. 1985;254:1480–4. Brett AS. Ethical issues in risk factor intervention. Am J Med. 1984;76:557–61. Sackett DL. The Doctor’s (Ethical and Moral) Dilemma. The Office of Health Economics Annual Lecture. The Office of Health Economics; London, England: 1996. Redelmeier DA, Tversky A. Discrepancy between medical decisions for individual patients and for groups. N Engl J Med. 1990;322(16):1162–4. Elstein AS. Clinical judgement: psychological research and medical practice. Science. 1976;194:696–700. McCormick J. The place of judgement in medicine. Br J Gen Pract. 1994;44:50–1. Nord E. The person trade-off approach to valuing health care programs. Med Decis Making. 1995;15:201–8. Malenka DJ, Baron JA. Cholesterol and coronary heart disease: the importance of patient-specific attributable risk. Arch Intern Med. 1988;148:2247–52. Malenka DJ, Baron JA. Cholesterol and coronary heart disease: the attributable risk reduction of diet and drugs. Arch Intern Med. 1989;149:1981–5. Laupacis A, Sackett DL, Roberts RS. An assessment of clinically useful measures of the consequences of treatment. N Engl J Med. 1988;318(26):1728–33. Laupacis A, Sackett DL, Roberts RS. Therapeutic priorities of Canadian internists. Can Med Assoc J. 1990;142(4):329–33. Reigelman R, Schroth WS. Adjusting the number needed to treat: incorporating adjustments for the utility and timing of benefits and harms. Med Decis Making. 1993;13:247–52. Sackett DL, Cook RJ. Understanding clinical trials. BMJ. 1994;304(6957):755–6. Sinclair JC, Bracken MB. Clinically useful measures of effect in binary analysis of randomised trials. J Clin Epidemiol. 1994;47(8):881–9. Cook RJ, Sackett DL. Number needed to treat: a clinically useful measure of treatment effect. BMJ. 1995;310(6977):452–4. Fahey T, Newton J. Conveying the benefits and risks of treatment. Br J Gen Pract. 1995;45(396):339–41. Guyatt GH, Jaeschke RZ, Cook DJ. Applying the findings of clinical trials to individual patients. ACP J Club. 1995;122(2):A12–3. Jaeschke R, Guyatt G, Shannon H, Walter S, Cook D, Heddle N. Basic statistics for clinicians, 3: assessing the effects of treatment: measures of association. Can Med Assoc J. 1995;152(3):351–7. Sackett DL. Applying overviews and meta analyses at the bedside. J Clin Epidemiol. 1995;48(1):61–6.