The neural basis of belief encoding and integration in moral judgment
Tài liệu tham khảo
Aichorn, 2006, Do visual perspective tasks need theory of mind?, NeuroImage, 30, 1059, 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.10.026
Baird, 2004, The role of mental state understanding in the development of moral cognition and moral action, New. Dir. Child. Adolesc. Dev., 103, 37, 10.1002/cd.96
Baird, 2001, Do preschoolers appreciate that identical actions may be motivated by different intentions?, J. Cogn. Dev., 2, 413, 10.1207/S15327647JCD0204_4
Borg, 2006, Consequences, action, and intention as factors in moral judgments: an FMRI investigation, J. Cogn. Neurosci., 18, 803, 10.1162/jocn.2006.18.5.803
Ciaramidaro, 2007, The intentional network: How the brain reads varieties of intentions, Neuropsychologia, 45, 3105, 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2007.05.011
Corbetta, 2000, Voluntary orienting is dissociated from target detection in human posterior parietal cortex, Nat. Neurosci., 3, 292, 10.1038/73009
Cushman, 2006, The role of conscious reasoning and intuitions in moral judgment: testing three principles of harm, Psychol. Sci., 17, 1082, 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2006.01834.x
Darley, 1982, Making moral judgment, Am. Sci., 70, 515
Decety, 2007, The role of the right temporoparietal junction in social interaction: how low-level computational processes contribute to meta-cognition, The Neuroscientist, 13, 580, 10.1177/1073858407304654
Dennett, 1978, Beliefs about beliefs, Behav. Brain Sci., 1, 568, 10.1017/S0140525X00076664
Fincham, 1979, Attribution of responsibility to the self and other in children and adults, J. Pers. Soc. Psychol., 37, 1589, 10.1037/0022-3514.37.9.1589
Flavell, 1999, Cognitive development: children’s knowledge about the mind, Ann. Rev. Psychol., 50, 21, 10.1146/annurev.psych.50.1.21
Fletcher, 1995, Other minds in the brain: a functional imaging study of “theory of mind” in story comprehension, Cognition, 57, 109, 10.1016/0010-0277(95)00692-R
Gallagher, 2000, Reading the mind in cartoons and stories: an fMRI study of ‘theory of mind’ in verbal and nonverbal tasks, Neuropsychologia, 38, 11, 10.1016/S0028-3932(99)00053-6
Gobbini, 2007, Two takes on the social brain: a comparison of theory of mind tasks, J. Cogn. Neurosci., 19, 1803, 10.1162/jocn.2007.19.11.1803
Greene, 2002, How (and where) does moral judgment work?, Trends Cogn. Sci., 6, 517, 10.1016/S1364-6613(02)02011-9
Grueneich, 1982, The development of children’s integration rules for making moral judgments, Child Dev., 53, 887, 10.2307/1129125
Hebble, 1971, Development of elementary school children’s judgment of intent, Child Dev., 42, 583, 10.2307/1127804
Karniol, 1978, Children’s use of intention cues in evaluating behavior, Psychol. Bull., 85, 76, 10.1037/0033-2909.85.1.76
Knobe, 2005, Theory of mind and moral cognition: exploring the connections., Trends Cogn. Sci., 9, 357, 10.1016/j.tics.2005.06.011
Mikhail, 2007, Universal moral grammar: theory, evidence and the future, Trends Cogn. Sci., 11, 143, 10.1016/j.tics.2006.12.007
Mitchell, 2007, Activity in right temporo-parietal junction is not selective for theory-of-mind, Cerebral Cortex, 18, 262, 10.1093/cercor/bhm051
Nelson Le Gall, 1985, Motive outcome matching and outcome foreseeability—effects on attribution of intentionality and moral judgments, Dev. Psychol., 21, 332, 10.1037/0012-1649.21.2.332
Nunez, 1998, Psychological and deontic concepts: separate domains or intimate connections, Mind Lang., 13, 153, 10.1111/1468-0017.00071
Onishi, 2005, Do 15-month-old infants understand false beliefs, Science, 308, 255, 10.1126/science.1107621
Perner, 2006, Thinking of mental and other representations: the roles of left and right temporo-parietal junction, Soc. Neurosci., 1, 245, 10.1080/17470910600989896
Piaget, J., 1965/1932. The Moral Judgment of the Child. Free Press, New York.
Ruby, 2003, What you belief versus what you think they believe: a neuroimaging study of conceptual perspective-taking, Eur. J. Neurosci., 17, 2475, 10.1046/j.1460-9568.2003.02673.x
Saxe, 2003, People thinking about thinking people. The role of the temporo-parietal junction in “theory of mind”, NeuroImage, 19, 1835, 10.1016/S1053-8119(03)00230-1
Saxe, 2006, It’s the thought that counts: Specific brain regions for one component of Theory of Mind, Psychol. Sci., 17, 692, 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2006.01768.x
Saxe, 2005, Making sense of another mind: The role of the right temporo-parietal junction, Neuropsychologia, 43, 1391, 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2005.02.013
Shultz, 1986, Assignment of moral responsibility and punishment, Child Dev., 57, 177, 10.2307/1130649
Siegel, 1998, Preschoolers’ understanding of lies and innocent and negligent mistakes, Dev. Psychol., 34, 332, 10.1037/0012-1649.34.2.332
Sommer, 2007, Neural correlates of true and false belief reasoning, NeuroImage, 35, 1378, 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.01.042
Vogeley, 2001, Mind reading: neural mechanisms of theory of mind and self-perspective., NeuroImage, 14, 170, 10.1006/nimg.2001.0789
Weiner, 1995
Wellman, 2001, Meta-analysis of theory-of-mind development: the truth about false belief, Child. Dev., 72, 655, 10.1111/1467-8624.00304
Wellman, 1979, Early development of moral criteria, Child Dev., 50, 869, 10.2307/1128956
Young, 2007, The neural basis of the interaction between theory of mind and moral judgment, Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci., 104, 8235, 10.1073/pnas.0701408104
Young, 2007, Investigating emotion in moral cognition: A review of evidence from functional neuroimaging and neuropsychology, Br. Med. Bull., 84, 69, 10.1093/bmb/ldm031
Yuill, 1984, Young children’s coordination of motive and outcome in judgements of satisfaction and morality, Br. J. Dev. Psychol., 2, 73, 10.1111/j.2044-835X.1984.tb00536.x
Yuill, 1988, Intentionality and knowledge in children’s judgments of actors responsibility and recipients emotional reaction, Dev. Psychol., 24, 358, 10.1037/0012-1649.24.3.358
Zelazo, 1996, Intention, act, and outcome in behavioral prediction and moral judgment, Child Dev., 67, 2478, 10.2307/1131635