Development of the appearance-reality distinction
Tài liệu tham khảo
Braine, 1965, The development of conservation of size, Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 4, 227, 10.1016/S0022-5371(65)80025-1
Braine, 1965, The conservation of a shape property and a proposal about the origin of the conservations, Canadian Journal of Psychology, 19, 197, 10.1037/h0082903
Carey, S. Are children fundamentally different kinds of thinkers and learners than adults? In S. E. Chipman, J. W. Segal, & R. Glaser (Eds.), Thinking and learning skills: Research and open questions. Hillsdale, N.J.: Erlbaum, in press. Vol. 2.
Daehler, 1970, Children's manipulation of illusory and ambiguous stimuli, discriminative performance, and implications for conceptual development, Child Development, 41, 225, 10.2307/1127402
De Vries, 1969, Constancy of generic identity in the years three to six, Society for Research in Child Development Monographs, 34, 10.2307/1165683
Elkind, 1966, Conservation across illusory transformations in young children, Acta Psychologica, 25, 389, 10.1016/0001-6918(66)90020-5
Flavell, 1963
Flavell, 1977
Flavell, 1981, Cognitive monitoring
Flavell, 1981, Young children's knowledge about visual perception: Further evidence for the Level 1-Level 2 distinction, Developmental Psychology, 17, 99, 10.1037/0012-1649.17.1.99
Freeman, 1977, What is the child's own view in a spatial task?: A spatial by temporal interaction affecting errors in perceptual reports, Perception, 6, 393, 10.1068/p060393
Freeman, 1980
Freeman, 1972, Intellectual realism in children's drawings of a familiar object with distinctive features, Child Development, 43, 1116, 10.2307/1127668
Gibson, 1950
Gleitman, H. Some trends in the study of cognition. In S. Koch & D. E. Leary (Eds.), A century of psychology as a science: Retrospections and assessments. New York: McGraw-Hill, in press.
Johnson, 1981, Reality monitoring, Psychological Review, 88, 67, 10.1037/0033-295X.88.1.67
Kielgast, 1971, Piaget's concept of spatial egocentrism: A re-evaluation, Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 12, 179, 10.1111/j.1467-9450.1971.tb00622.x
King, 1971, A nonarbitrary behavioral criterion for conservation of illusion-distorted length in five-year-olds, Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 11, 171, 10.1016/0022-0965(71)90073-7
Langer, 1972, Appearance, reality, and identity, Cognition, 1, 105, 10.1016/0010-0277(72)90048-0
Liben, 1978, Perspective-taking skills in young children: Seeing the world through rose-colored glasses, Developmental Psychology, 14, 87, 10.1037/0012-1649.14.1.87
Liben, 1981, Intellectual realism: Implications for investigations of perceptual perspective-taking in young children, Child Development, 52, 921, 10.2307/1129095
Light, 1981, Internal spatial relationships in young children's drawings, Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 31, 521, 10.1016/0022-0965(81)90034-5
Light, 1980, Depth relationships in young children's drawings, Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 30, 79, 10.1016/0022-0965(80)90076-4
Luquet, 1927
Murray, 1965, Conservation of illusion-distorted lengths and areas by primary school children, Journal of Educational Psychology, 56, 62, 10.1037/h0021736
Murray, 1968, Phenomenal-real discrimination and the conservation of illusion-distorted length, Canadian Journal of Psychology, 22, 114, 10.1037/h0082748
Olson, D. R., & Hildyard, A. Writing and literal meaning. In M. Martlew (Ed.), Psychology of writing. New York: Wiley, in press.
Piaget, 1968
Piaget, 1956
Piaget, 1969
Potter, 1979, Mundane symbolism: The relations among objects, names, and ideas
Taylor, 1982, Constraints on the visual accuracy of drawings produced by young children, Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 33, 311, 10.1016/0022-0965(82)90049-2
Tronick, 1979, Size-distance perception in preschool children, Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 27, 166, 10.1016/0022-0965(79)90068-7
Flavell, 1976, The development of metacommunication