The phonological loop and the irrelevant speech effect: Some comments on Neath (2000)
Tóm tắt
Neath (2000) presents a useful overview of the evidence to be explained by any model of the effects of irrelevant speech on immediate serial memory and proposes a model accompanied by computational simulation. While his review is in general accurate, it is limited in its explanation of the crucial characteristics of the disrupting sounds. It also neglects strategic issues, particularly the tendency for subjects to switch strategy as list length increases. As a result, his model fails to account for the absence of an interaction between irrelevant speech and acoustic similarity for lists of span length. Points of issue between Neath’s feature hypothesis and the phonological loop interpretation are outlined, and the contribution of his computational simulation is discussed.
Tài liệu tham khảo
Atkinson, R. C., &Shiffrin, R. M. (1968). Human memory: A proposed system and its control processes. In K. W. Spence & J. T. Spence (Eds.),The psychology of learning and motivation: Advances in research and theory (Vol. 2, pp. 89–195). New York: Academic Press.
Baddeley, A. D. (1966a). Short-term memory for word sequences as a function of acoustic, semantic and formal similarity.Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology,18, 362–365.
Baddeley, A. D. (1966b). The influence of acoustic and semantic similarity on long-term memory for word sequences.Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology,18, 302–309.
Baddeley, A. D. (1986).Working memory. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Baddeley, A. D. (1992). Working memory.Science,255, 556–559.
Baddeley, A. D., &Ecob, J. R. (1970). Simultaneous acoustic and semantic coding in short-term memory.Nature,277, 288–289.
Baddeley, A. D., Gathercole, S. E., &Papagno, C. (1998). The phonological loop as a language learning device.Psychological Review,105, 158–173.
Baddeley, A. D., &Hitch, G. J. (1974). Working memory. In G. H. Bower (Ed.),The psychology of learning and motivation (pp. 47–89). New York: Academic Press.
Baddeley, A. D., Lewis, V. J., &Vallar, G. (1984). Exploring the articulatory loop.Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology,36A, 233–252.
Baddeley, A. D., &Salamé, P. (1986). The unattended speech effect: Perception or memory?Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition,12, 525–529.
Baddeley, A. D., Thomson, N., &Buchanan, M. (1975). Word length and the structure of short-term memory.Journal of Verbal Learning & Verbal Behavior,14, 575–589.
Burgess, N., &Hitch, G. J. (1992). Towards a network model of the articulatory loop.Journal of Memory & Language,31, 429–460.
Burgess, N., &Hitch, G. J. (1999). Memory for serial order: A network model of the phonological loop and its timing.Psychological Review,106, 551–581.
Chincotta, D.,Baddeley, A. D., &Stafford, L. (2000).Is the word length in STM attributable to output delay? Evidence from serial recognition. Manuscript submitted for publication.
Colle, H. A. (1980). Auditory encoding in visual short-term recall: Effects of noise intensity and spatial location.Journal of Verbal Learning & Verbal Behavior,19, 722–735.
Colle, H. A., &Welsh, A. (1976). Acoustic masking in primary memory.Journal of Verbal Learning & Verbal Behavior,15, 17–32.
Conrad, R., &Hull, A. J. (1964). Information, acoustic confusion and memory span.British Journal of Psychology,55, 429–432.
Cowan, N., Day, L., Saults, J. S., Keller, T. A., Johnson, T., &Flores, L. (1992). The role of verbal output time and the effects of word length on immediate memory.Journal of Memory & Language,31, 1–17.
Cowan, N., Nugent, L. D., Elliott, E. M., &Geer, T. (2000). Is there a temporal basis of the word length effect? A response to Service (1998).Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology,53A, 647–660.
Ellermeier, W., &Hellbruck, J. (1998). Is level irrelevant in “irrelevant speech”? Effects of loudness, signal-to-noise ratio, and binaural masking.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance,24, 1406–1414.
Ellermeier, W., &Zimmer, K. (1997). Individual differences in susceptibility to the “irrelevant speech” effect.Journal of the Acoustical Society of America,102, 2191–2199.
Hall, J. W., Wilson, K. P., Humphreys, M. S., Tinzmann, M. B., &Bowyer, P. M. (1983). Phonemic-similarity effects in good versus poor readers.Memory & Cognition,11, 520–527.
Henson, R. N. A. (1998). Short-term memory for serial order. The start-end model.Cognitive Psychology,36, 73–137.
Johnston, R. S. (1982). Phonological coding in dyslexic readers.British Journal of Psychology,73, 455–460.
Johnston, R. S., Rugg, M. E., &Scott, T. (1987). Phonological similarity effects, memory span and developmental reading disorders: The nature of the relationship.British Journal of Psychology,78, 205–211.
Jones, D. M. (1993). Objects, streams and threads of auditory attention. In A. D. Baddeley & L. Weiskrantz (Eds.),Attention: Selection, awareness and control (pp. 87–104). Oxford: Oxford University Press, Clarendon Press.
Jones, D. M., &Macken, W. J. (1993). Irrelevant tones produce an irrelevant-speech effect: Implications for phonological coding in working memory.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition,19, 369–381.
Jones, D. M., &Macken, W. J. (1995a). Auditory babble and cognitive efficiency: Role and number of voices and their location.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied,1, 216–226.
Jones, D. M., &Macken, W. J. (1995b). Phonological similarity in the irrelevant speech effect. Within- or between-stream similarity?Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition,21, 103–115.
Jones, D. M., &Tremblay, S. (2000). Interference in memory by process or content? A reply to Neath (2000).Psychonomic Bulletin & Review,7, 550–558.
Larsen, J. D., Baddeley, A. D., &Andrade, J. (2000). Phonological similarity and the irrelevant speech effect: Implications for models of short-term verbal memory.Memory,8, 145–158.
LeCompte, D. C., &Shaibe, D. M. (1997). On the irrelevance of phonological similarity to the irrelevant-speech effect.Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology,50A, 100–118.
Mann, V. A., Liberman, I. Y., &Shankweiler, D. (1980). Children’s memory for sentences and word strings in relation to reading ability.Memory & Cognition,8, 329–335.
Murray, D. J. (1968). Articulation and acoustic confusability in shortterm memory.Journal of Experimental Psychology,78, 679–684.
Neath, I. (2000). Modeling the effects of irrelevant speech on memory.Psychonomic Bulletin & Review,7, 403–423.
Neath, I., Surprenant, A. M., &LeCompte, D. C. (1998). Irrelevant speech eliminates the word length effect.Memory & Cognition,26, 343–354.
Page, M. P. A., &Norris, D. (1998). The primacy model: A new model of immediate serial recall.Psychological Review,105, 761–781.
Robbins, T., Anderson, E., Barker, D., Bradley, A., Fearneyhough, C., Henson, R., Hudson, S., &Baddeley, A. (1996). Working memory in chess.Memory & Cognition,24, 83–93.
Saariluoma, P. (1995).Chess players’ thinking: A cognitive psychological approach. London: Routledge.
Salamé, P., &Baddeley, A. D. (1982). Disruption of short-term memory by unattended speech: Implications for the structure of working memory.Journal of Verbal Learning & Verbal Behavior,21, 150–164.
Salamé, P., &Baddeley, A. D. (1983). Differential effects of noise and speech on short-term memory. InProceedings of the Fourth International Congress on Noise as a Public Health Problem (pp. 751–758). Milan: Centro Ricerche e Studi Amplifon.
Salamé, P., &Baddeley, A. D. (1986). Phonological factors in STM: Similarity and the unattended speech effect.Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society,24, 263–265.
Salamé, P., &Baddeley, A. D. (1987). Noise, unattended speech and short-term memory.Ergonomics,30, 1185–1194.
Salamé, P., &Baddeley, A. D. (1989). Effects of background music on phonological short-term memory.Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology,41A, 107–122.
Service, E. (1998). Effect of word length on immediate serial recall depends on phonological complexity, not articulatory duration.Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology,51A, 283–304.
Shankweiler, D., Liberman, I. Y., Mark, L. S., Fowler, C. A., &Fischer, F. W. (1979). The speech code and learning to read.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning & Memory,5, 531–545.
Smith, E. E., &Jonides, J. (1995). Working memory in humans: Neuropsychological evidence. In M. Gazzaniga (Ed.),The cognitive neurosciences (pp. 1009–1020). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Tremblay, S., &Jones, D. M. (1998). Role of habituation in the irrelevant sound effect: Evidence from the effects of token set size and rate of transition.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition,24, 659–671.
Vallar, G., &Baddeley, A. D. (1984). Fractionation of working memory. Neuropsychological evidence for a phonological shortterm store.Journal of Verbal Learning & Verbal Behavior,23, 151–161.
Waters, G. S., &Caplan, D. (1996). The measurement of verbal working memory capacity and its relation to reading comprehension.Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology,49A, 51–79.